Show Summary Details

Page of

PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 19 November 2019

(p. 640) References

(p. 640) References

Abler, W. (1989). On the particulate principle of self‐diversifying systems. Journal of Social and Biological Structures 12: 1–13.Find this resource:

Aboitiz, F., Garcia, R., Bosman, C., and Brunetti, E. (2006). Cortical memory mechanisms and language origins. Brain and Language 98: 40–56.Find this resource:

Adams, A.‐M. and Gathercole, S. E. (2000). Limitations in working memory: Implications for language development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 35: 95–116.Find this resource:

Aiello, L. C. (1996). Terrestriality, bipedalism and the origin of language. Proceedings of the British Academy 88: 269–289.Find this resource:

Aiello, L. C. (1998). The foundation of human language. In Jablonski and Aiello (eds.), 21–34.Find this resource:

Aiello, L. C. and Dean, M. C. (1990). An introduction to human evolutionary anatomy. London: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Aiello, L. C. and Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Neocortex size, group size and the evolution of language. Current Anthropology 34: 184–193.Find this resource:

Aiello, L. C. and Wheeler, P. (1995). The expensive‐tissue hypothesis: The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current Anthropology 36: 199–221.Find this resource:

Aiello, L. C., Bates, N., and Joffe, T. (2001). In defense of the extensive tissue hypothesis. In Falk and Gibson (eds.), 57–78.Find this resource:

Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2002). Language contact in Amazonia. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Aitchison, J. (1996). The seeds of speech: Language origin and evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Alemseged, Z., Spoor, F., Kimbel, W. H., Bobe, R., Geraads, D., Reed, D., and Wynn, J. G. (2006). A juvenile early hominin skeleton from Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature 443: 296–301.Find this resource:

Alexander, R. D. (1989). Evolution of the human psyche. In Mellars and Stringer (eds.), 455–513.Find this resource:

Alexeev, V. (1986). The origin of the human race. Moscow: Progress Publishers.Find this resource:

Alley, T. R. (1980). Infantile coloration as an elicitor of caretaking behavior in old‐world primates. Primates 21: 416–429.Find this resource:

Alon, U. (2007). An introduction to systems biology. New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC.Find this resource:

Alperson‐Afil, N., Richter, D., and Goren‐Inbar, N. (2007). Phantom hearths and the use of fire at Gesher Benot Ya'aqov, Israel. PaleoAnthropology 2007: 1–15.Find this resource:

Ambrose, S. H. (2001). Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science 291: 1748–1753.Find this resource:

Ambrose, S. H. (2008). Coevolution of composite tool technology, working memory and language: Implications for the evolution of modern human behavior. Paper presented at the Wenner‐Gren Foundation Symposium 139: Working Memory and the Evolution of Modern Thinking, Cascais, Portugal. (p. 641) Find this resource:

Amunts, K., Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Steinmetz, H., Schleicher, A., Dabringhaus, A., and Zilles, K. (1997). Motor cortex and hand motor skills: Structural compliance in the human brain. Human Brain Mapping 5: 206–215.Find this resource:

Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., Burgel, U., Mohlberg, H., Uylings, H. B., and Zilles, K. (1999). Broca's region revisited: Cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. Journal of Comparative Neurology 412: 319–341.Find this resource:

Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Find this resource:

Anderson, S. R. (2004). Doctor Dolittle's delusion: Animals and the uniqueness of human language. New Haven, CT and London: Yale University Press.Find this resource:

Anderson, S. R. (2008). The logical structure of linguistic theory. Language 84: 795–814.Find this resource:

Anderson, S. R. and Lightfoot, D. W. (2002). The language organ: Linguistics as cognitive physiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Andrew, R. J. (2002). The earliest origins and subsequent evolution of lateralization. In L. J. Rogers and R. J. Andrew (eds.), Comparative vertebrate lateralization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 70–93.Find this resource:

Apicella, C. L., Feinberg, D. R., and Marlowe, F. W. (2007). Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter‐gatherers. Biology Letters 3: 682–684.Find this resource:

Arambourg, C. and Coppens, Y. (1968). Decouverte d'un australopithecien nouveau dans les Gisements de L'Omo (Ethiopie). South African Journal of Science 64: 58–59.Find this resource:

Arbib, M. A. (2002). The mirror system, imitation, and the evolution of language. In Dautenhahn and Nehaniv (eds.), 229–280.Find this resource:

Arbib, M. A. (2003). The evolving mirror system: A neural basis for language readiness. In Christiansen and Kirby (eds.), 182–200.Find this resource:

Arbib, M. A. (2005a). From monkey‐like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 105–167.Find this resource:

Arbib, M. A. (2005b). Interweaving protosign and protospeech: Further developments beyond the mirror. Interaction Studies 6: 145–171.Find this resource:

Arbib, M. A. (ed.) (2006). Action to language via the mirror neuron system. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Arbib, M. A. (2008). From grasp to language: Embodied concepts and the challenge of abstraction. Journal of Physiology Paris 102: 4–20.Find this resource:

Arbib, M. A. (2009). Invention and community in the emergence of language: A perspective from new sign languages. In S. M. Platek and T. K. Shackelford (eds.), Foundations in evolutionary cognitive neuroscience: Introduction to the discipline. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 117–152.Find this resource:

Arensburg, B., Tillier, A. M., Vandermeersch, B., Duday, H., Schepartz, L. A., and Rak, Y. (1989). A Middle Palaeolithic human hyoid bone. Nature 338: 758–760.Find this resource:

Arensburg, B., Schepartz, L. A., Tillier, A. M., Vandermeersch, B., and Rak, Y. (1990). A reappraisal of the anatomical basis for speech in Middle Palaeolithic hominids. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 83: 137–146.Find this resource:

Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K., and Pika, S. (2008). Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Current Anthropology 49: 1053–1076.Find this resource:

Ariew, A. (1999). Innateness is canalization: In defense of a developmental account of innateness. In V. G. Hardcastle (ed.), Where biology meets psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 117–138.Find this resource:

Aristotle (1984). The complete works of Aristotle. Edited by Jonathan Barnes. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Citations from On interpretation, On soul, Prior analytics.) (p. 642) Find this resource:

Armstrong, D. F. and Wilcox, S. E. (2007). The gestural origin of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006a). Language evolution: Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature 441: 303.Find this resource:

Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006b). The alarm‐calling system of adult male putty‐nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Animal Behaviour 72: 643–653.Find this resource:

Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2008). Meaningful call combinations in a non‐human primate. Current Biology 18: R202–R203.Find this resource:

Arnold, K., Pohlner, Y., and Zuberbühler, K. (2008). A forest monkey's alarm call series to predator models. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 549–559.Find this resource:

Aronoff, M., Meir, I., Padden, C., and Sandler, W. (2004). Morphological universals and the sign language type. In G. Booij and J. van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of morphology 2004. Dordrecht and Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 19–39.Find this resource:

Aronov, D., Andalman, A. S., and Fee, M. S. (2008). A specialized forebrain circuit for vocal babbling in the juvenile songbird. Science 320: 630–634.Find this resource:

Asfaw, B., White, T., Lovejoy, O., Latimer, B., Simpson, S., and Suwa, G. (1999). Australopithecus garhi: A new species of early hominid from Ethiopia. Science 284: 629–635.Find this resource:

Ashby, W. R. (1947). Principles of the self‐organizing dynamic system. Journal of General Psychology 37: 125–128.Find this resource:

Au, W. W. L. (1993). The sonar of dolphins. New York: Springer.Find this resource:

Austerlitz, R. (1980). Language‐family density in North America and Eurasia. Ural‐Altaische Jahrbücher 51: 1–10.Find this resource:

Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Austin, P. and Bresnan, J. (1996). Non‐configurationality in Australian Aboriginal languages. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 14: 215–268.Find this resource:

Avise, J. C. (2005). Molecular markers, natural history and evolution (2nd revised edn.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates Inc.Find this resource:

Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 417–423.Find this resource:

Baddeley, A. D. (2001). Is working memory still working? American Psychologist 11: 851–864.Find this resource:

Baddeley, A. D. and Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press, 47–90.Find this resource:

Baddeley, A. D. and Logie, R. H. (1999). Working memory: The multiple‐component model. In A. Miyake and P. Shah (eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York: Cambridge University Press, 28–61.Find this resource:

Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S., and Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review 105: 158–173.Find this resource:

Bae, C. J. (2010). The late Middle Pleistocene hominin fossil record of Eastern Asia: Synthesis and review. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 53: 75–93.Find this resource:

Bailey, D. H. and Geary, D. C. (2009). Hominid brain structure: Testing climatic, ecological and social competition models. Human Nature 20: 67–79.Find this resource:

Bailey, P., von Bonin, G., and McCulloch, W. S. (1950). The isocortex of the chimpanzee. Urbana‐Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.Find this resource:

Baker, M. C. (2001). The atoms of language: The mind's hidden rules of grammar. New York: Basic Books. (p. 643) Find this resource:

Baker, P. (1990). Off target? Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 5: 107–119.Find this resource:

Baker, P. (1997). Directionality in pidginization and creolization. In A. K. Spears and D. Winford (eds.), The structure and status of pidgins and creoles. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 91–109.Find this resource:

Baker, P. (2000). Theories of creolization and the degree and nature of restructuring. In I. Neumann‐Holzschuh and E. W. Schneider (eds.), Degrees of restructuring in creole languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 41–63.Find this resource:

Bakker, P. (1987). Autonomous languages: Signed and spoken languages created by children in the light of Bickerton's bioprogram hypothesis. Publikaties van het Instituut voor Algemene Taalwetenschap 53. Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam.Find this resource:

Bakker, P. (2006). Twins' languages as windows on language genesis. Paper presented at the Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study, Wassenaar, 13 June 2006.Find this resource:

Balaban, M. and Waxman, S. (1992). Words may facilitate categorization in 9‐month‐old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 64: 3–26.Find this resource:

Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development 62: 875–890.Find this resource:

Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore and P. J. Dunham (eds.), Joint attention. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 131–158.Find this resource:

Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and evolution. New York: Macmillan.Find this resource:

Ball, P. (1999). The self‐made tapestry. New York: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Ballentijn, M. R. and ten Cate, C. (1999). Variation in the number of elements in the perch‐coo vocalization of the collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto) and what it may tell about the sender. Behaviour 136: 847–864.Find this resource:

Balter, M. (2005). Are human brains still evolving? Brain genes show signs of selection. Science 309: 1662–1663.Find this resource:

Barham, L. (2002). Systematic pigment use in the Middle Pleistocene of South‐Central Africa. Current Anthropology 43: 181–190.Find this resource:

Baronchelli, A., Loreto, V., and Steels, L. (2008). In‐depth analysis of the naming game dynamics: The homogeneous mixing case. International Journal of Modern Physics C 19: 785–812.Find this resource:

Baron‐Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Batali, J. (1998). Computational simulations of the emergence of grammar. In Hurford et al. (eds.), 405–426.Find this resource:

Batali, J. (2002). The negotiation and acquisition of recursive grammars as a result of competition among exemplars. In Briscoe (ed.), 111–172.Find this resource:

Bates, E., Thal, D., and Marchman, V. (1991). Symbols and syntax: A Darwinian approach to language development. In Krasnegor et al. (eds.), 29–65.Find this resource:

Bateson, P. (2002). The corpse of a wearisome debate. Science 297: 2212–2213.Find this resource:

Bateson, P. (2004). The origins of human differences. Daedalus 133: 36–46.Find this resource:

Bazin, E., Glémier, S., and Galtier, N. (2006). Population size does not influence mitochondrial genetic diversity in animals. Science 312: 570–572.Find this resource:

Bearzi, M. and Stanford, C. (2010). A bigger, better brain. American Scientist 98: 402–409.Find this resource:

Beckers, G. J. L., Nelson, B., and Suthers, R. A. (2004). Vocal‐tract filtering by lingual articulation in a parrot. Current Biology 14: 1592–1597. (p. 644) Find this resource:

Bednarik, R. (2008). The mythical moderns. Journal of World Prehistory 21: 85–102.Find this resource:

Beecher, M. D. and Campbell, S. E. (2005). The role of unshared songs in singing interactions between neighbouring song sparrows. Animal Behaviour 70: 1297–1304.Find this resource:

Beecher M. D., Campbell, S. E., Burt, J. M., Hill, C. E., and Nordby, J. C. (2000). Song‐type matching between neighbouring song sparrows. Animal Behaviour 59: 21–27.Find this resource:

Behar, D. M., Villems, R., Soodyall, H., Blue‐Smith, J., Pereira, L., Metspalu, E., Scozzari, R., Makkan, H., Tzur, S., Comas, D., Bertranpetit, J., Quintana‐Murci, L., Tyler‐Smith, C., Spencer Wells, R., Rosset, S., and The Genographic Consortium. (2008). The dawn of human matrilineal diversity. American Journal of Human Genetics 82: 1130–1140.Find this resource:

Benazzo, S. (2009). The emergence of temporality: From restricted linguistic systems to early human language. In Botha and de Swart (eds.), 21–57.Find this resource:

Bengtson, J. and Ruhlen, M. (1994). Global etymologies. In M. Ruhlen, On the origin of languages: Studies in linguistic taxonomy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 277–329.Find this resource:

Bennett, E. I., Diamond, M. C., Krech, D., and Rosenzwieg, M. R. (1964). Chemical and anatomical plasticity of the brain. Science 146: 610–619.Find this resource:

Benton, M. J. and Donoghue, P. C. (2007). Palaeontological evidence to date the tree of life. Molecular Biology and Evolution 25: 26–53.Find this resource:

Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (2003). Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science 302: 1234–1236.Find this resource:

Berk, L. (1999). Children's private speech: An overview of theory and the status of research. In P. Lloyd and C. Fernyhough (eds.), Lev Vygotsky: Critical assessments. Vol II, Thought and language. Florence, KY: Taylor & Francis/Routledge, 33–70.Find this resource:

Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word 14: 150–177.Find this resource:

Berrah, A.‐R. and Laboissière, R. (1999). Species: An evolutionary model for the emergence of phonetic structures in an artificial society of speech agents. In D. Floreano, J.‐D. Nicoud, and F. Mondada (eds.), Advances in artificial life: Lecture notes in artificial intelligence Vol. 1674. Berlin: Springer Verlag, 674–678.Find this resource:

Bersaglieri, T., Sabeti, P. C., Patterson, N., Vanderploeg, T., Schaffner, S. F., Drake, J. A., Rhodes, M., Reich, D. E., and Hirschhorn, J. N. (2004). Genetic signatures of strong recent positive selection at the Lactase gene. American Journal of Human Genetics 74: 1111–1120.Find this resource:

Biben, M. and Symmes, D. (1986). Play vocalizations of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Folia Primatologica 46: 173–182.Find this resource:

Bickel, B. (2008). A general method for the statistical evaluation of typological universals. Ms, University of Leipzig. www.uni‐leipzig.de/~bickel (accessed February 15, 2010).

Bickel, B. and Nichols, J. (2002). The AUTOTYP research program. http://www.uni‐leipzig.de/~autotyp/

Bickel, B. and Nichols, J. (2005). Inclusive/exclusive as person vs. number categories worldwide. In E. Filimonova (ed.), Clusivity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 47–70.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7: 173–221.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (1988). Seselwa serialization and its significance. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 4: 155–183.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (p. 645) Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (1995). Language and human behavior. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press/London: UCL Press.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (1998). Catastrophic evolution: The case for a single step from protolanguage to full human language. In Hurford et al. (eds.), 341–358.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (2000). How protolanguage became language. In Knight et al. (eds.), 264–284.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (2003). Symbol and structure: A comprehensive framework for language evolution. In Christiansen and Kirby (eds.), 77–93.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (2007). Language evolution: A brief guide for linguists. Lingua 117: 510–526.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (2008). Bastard tongues. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (2009a). Adam's tongue: How humans made language, how language made humans. New York: Hill and Wang/Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (2009b). Recursion: Core of complexity or artifact of analysis? In T. Givón and M. Shibatani (eds.), Syntactic complexity: Diachrony, acquisition, neuro‐cognition, evolution. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 531–544.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (2009c). Syntax for non‐syntacticians: A brief primer. In Bickerton and Szathmáry (eds.), 3–13.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. (2010). On two incompatible theories of language evolution. In Larson et al. (eds.), 199–210.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. and Odo, C. (1976). General phonology and pidgin syntax. Final Report on NSF Grant No. 39748. University of Hawaii.Find this resource:

Bickerton, D. and Szathmáry, E. (eds.) (2009). Biological foundations and origins of syntax. Strüngmann Forum Reports, Vol. 3. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Bird, C. D. and Emery, N. J. (2009). Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool‐using rooks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 106: 10370–10375.Find this resource:

Birdsell, J. B. (1953). Some environmental and cultural factors influencing the structuring of Australian Aboriginal populations. American Naturalist 87: 171–207.Find this resource:

Birdsell, J. B. (1957). Some population problems involving Pleistocene man. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology 22: 47–69.Find this resource:

Bisang, W. (2004). Grammaticalization without co‐evolution of form and meaning: The case of tense‐aspect‐modality in East and Mainland Southeast Asia. In Bisang et al. (eds.), 109–138.Find this resource:

Bisang, W. (2009). On the evolution of complexity – sometimes less is more in East and mainland Southeast Asia. In Sampson et al. (eds.), 19–33.Find this resource:

Bisang, W., Himmelmann, N., and Wiemer, B. (eds.) (2004). What makes grammaticalization? A look from its fringes and its components. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Find this resource:

Blacking, J. (1973). How musical is man? Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Find this resource:

Blakemore, S. and Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47: 296–312.Find this resource:

Bleed, P. (1986). The optimal design of hunting weapons: Maintainability or reliability? American Antiquity 51: 737–747.Find this resource:

Blevins, J. (1995). The syllable in phonological theory. In J. Goldsmith (ed.), Handbook of phonological theory. Oxford: Blackwell, 206–244.Find this resource:

Blevins, J. (2004). Evolutionary phonology: The emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (p. 646) Find this resource:

Blondin‐Massé, A., Chicoisne, G., Gargouri, Y., Harnad, S., Picard, O., and Marcotte, O. (2008). How is meaning grounded in dictionary definitions? In Proceedings of the 3rd Textgraphs Workshop on Graph‐Based Algorithms for Natural Language Processing, 17–24.Find this resource:

Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Henry Holt and Co.Find this resource:

Boeckx, C. (2006). Linguistic minimalism: Origins, concepts, methods, and aims. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Boeckx, C. (2008). Did we really solve Plato's Problem (abstractly)? In Y. Otsu (ed.), Proceedings of the 9th Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics. Tokyo: Hituzi Syobo, 1–25.Find this resource:

Boeckx, C. (2009). Linguistic minimalism. In B. Heine and H. Narrog (eds.), Oxford handbook of grammatical analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 537–562.Find this resource:

Boeckx, C. (2011). Some reflections on Darwin's problem in the context of Cartesian Biolinguistics. In A.‐M. di Sciullo and C. Boeckx (eds.), The biolinguistic enterprise: New perspectives on the evolution and nature of the human language faculty. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 42–64.Find this resource:

Boeckx, C. and Grohmann, K. K. (2007). The Biolinguistics manifesto. Biolinguistics 1: 1–8.Find this resource:

Boehm, C. (2001). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 78: 547–573.Find this resource:

Boesch, C. and Boesch‐Achermann, H. (2000). The chimpanzees of the Taï forest: Behavioral ecology and evolution. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Bolhuis, J. J. and Gahr, M. (2006). Neural mechanisms of birdsong memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7: 347–357.Find this resource:

Boogert, N. J., Giraldeau, L‐A., and Lefebvre, L. (2008). Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males. Animal Behaviour 76: 1735–1741.Find this resource:

Booth, A. E. and Waxman, S. R. (2002). Object names and object functions serve as cues to categories in infancy. Developmental Psychology 38: 948–957.Find this resource:

Boran, J. R. and Heimlich, S. (1999). Social learning in cetaceans: Hunting, hearing and hierarchies. In Box and Gibson (eds.), 282–307.Find this resource:

Borden, G. J., Harris, K. S., and Raphael, L. J. (2003). Speech science primer: Physiology, acoustics, and perception of speech. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins.Find this resource:

Bordes, F. (1968). The Old Stone Age. New York: World University Library.Find this resource:

Borg, E. (2007). If mirror neurons are the answer, what was the question? Journal of Consciousness Studies 14: 5–19.Find this resource:

Botha, R. (2003). Unravelling the evolution of language. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Find this resource:

Botha, R. (2006a). On the windows approach to language evolution. Language and Communication 26: 129–43.Find this resource:

Botha, R. (2006b). Pidgin languages as a putative window on language evolution. Language and Communication 26: 1–14.Find this resource:

Botha R. (2008). Prehistoric shell beads as a window on language evolution. Language and Communication 28: 197–212.Find this resource:

Botha, R. (2009a). On musilanguage/‘Hmmmmm’ as an evolutionary precursor to language. Language and Communication 29: 61–76.Find this resource:

Botha, R. (2009b). Theoretical underpinnings of inferences about language evolution: The syntax used at Blombos Cave. In Botha and Knight (eds.), (2009a), 93–111. (p. 647) Find this resource:

Botha R. (2009c). What are windows on language evolution? In Botha and de Swart (eds.), 1–25.Find this resource:

Botha R., and de Swart, H. (eds.) (2009). Language evolution: The view from restricted linguistic systems. Utrecht: LOT.Find this resource:

Botha R. and Knight, C. (eds.) (2009a). The cradle of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Botha, R. and Knight, C. (eds.) (2009b). The prehistory of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Boughman, J. W. (1998). Vocal learning by greater spear‐nosed bats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 265: 227–233.Find this resource:

Bowern, C. and Koch, H. (2004). Australian languages: Classification and the comparative method. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Find this resource:

Bowie, J. (2008). Proto‐discourse and the emergence of compositionality. Interaction Studies 9: 18–33.Find this resource:

Box, H. O. and Gibson, K. R. (eds.) (1999). Mammalian social learning: Comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Boyd, R. and Silk, J. B. (2003). How humans evolved. New York: W. W. Norton.Find this resource:

Boyer, P. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Find this resource:

Boyland, J. T. (1996). Morphosyntactic change in progress: A psycholinguistic approach. Unpublished PhD thesis. Berkeley, CA: University of California.Find this resource:

Brain, C. K. and Sillen, A. (1988). Evidence from the Swartkrans cave for the earliest use of fire. Nature 336: 464–466.Find this resource:

Braine, M. D. (1994). Is nativism sufficient? Journal of Child Language 21: 9–31.Find this resource:

Bramble, D. M. and Lieberman, D. E. (2004). Endurance running in the evolution of Homo. Nature 432: 345–352.Find this resource:

Brantingham, P. J., Kuhn, S. L., and Kerry, K.W. (eds.) (2004). The early Upper Paleolithic beyond Western Europe. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Find this resource:

Brighton, H. (2002). Compositional syntax from cultural transmission. Artificial Life 8: 25–54.Find this resource:

Brighton, H., Kirby, S., and Smith, K. (2005a). Cultural selection for learnability: Three principles underlying the view that language adapts to be learnable. In Tallerman (ed.), 291–333.Find this resource:

Brighton, H., Smith, K., and Kirby, S. (2005b). Language as an evolutionary system. Physics of Life Reviews 2: 177–226.Find this resource:

Briscoe, T. (2000). Grammatical acquisition: Inductive bias and coevolution of language and the language acquisition device. Language 76: 245–296.Find this resource:

Briscoe, T. (ed.) (2002). Linguistic evolution through language acquisition: Formal and computational models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Briscoe, T. (2009). What can formal or computational models tell us about how (much) language shaped the brain? In Bickerton and Szathmáry (eds.), 369–382.Find this resource:

Broaders, S., Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., and Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2007). Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136: 539–550. (p. 648) Find this resource:

Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé; suivies d'une observation d'aphémie. Bulletin de la Société Anatomique 6: 330–357.Find this resource:

Broom, R. (1938). The Pleistocene anthropoid apes of South Africa. Nature 142: 377–379.Find this resource:

Browman, C. P. and Goldstein, L. (1992). Articulatory phonology: An overview. Phonetica 49: 155–180.Find this resource:

Brown, J. C. (1966). Loglan: A logical language. Gainesville, FL: The Loglan Institute.Find this resource:

Brown, K. S., Marean, C. W., Herries, A. I. R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Braun, D., Roberts, D. L., Meyer, M. C., and Bernatchez, J. (2009). Fire as an engineering tool of early modern humans. Science 325: 859–862.Find this resource:

Brown, S. (2000). The ‘musilanguage’ model of human evolution. In Wallin et al. (eds.), 271–300.Find this resource:

Brunet, M., Beauvilain, A., Coppens, Y., Heintz, E., Moutaye, A. H. E., and Pilbeam, D. (1996). Australopithecus bahrelghazali, une nouvelle espèce d'Hominide ancien de la région de Koro Toro (Tchad). Comptes rendus de l'Académie des sciences 322: 907–913.Find this resource:

Brunet, M., Guy, F., Pilbeam, D., Mackaye, H. T., Likius, A., Ahounta, D., Beauvilain, A., Blondel, C., Bocherens, H., Boisserie, J.‐R., De Bonis, L., Coppens, Y., Dejax, J., Denys, C., Duringer, P., Eisenmann, V., Gongdibe, F., Fronty, P., Geraads, D., Lehmann, T., Lihoreau, F., Louchart, A., Mahamat, A., Merceron, G., Mouchelin, G., Otero, O., Campomanes, P. P., De Leon, M. P., Rage, J.‐C., Sapanet, M., Schuster, M., Sudre, J., Tassy, P., Valentin, X., Vignaud, P., Viriot, L., Zazzo, A., and Zollikofer, C. (2002). A new hominid from the Upper Miocene of Chad, Central Africa. Nature 418: 145–151.Find this resource:

Bugnyar, T. and Kotrschal, K. (2002). Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: Is it tactical deception? Animal Behaviour 64: 185–195.Find this resource:

Burling, R. (1993). Primate calls, human language, and nonverbal communication. Current Anthropology 34: 25–53.Find this resource:

Burling, R. (2002). The slow growth of language in children. In Wray (ed.), 297–310.Find this resource:

Burling, R. (2005). The talking ape: How language evolved. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Burt, J. M., Bard, S. C., Campbell, S. E., and Beecher, M. D. (2002). Alternative forms of song matching in song sparrows. Animal Behaviour 63: 1143–1151.Find this resource:

Buss, D. M. (2003). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. Revised edition. New York: Basic Books.Find this resource:

Bustamante, C. D., Fledel‐Alon, A., Williamson, S., Nielsen, R., Hubisz, M. T., Glanowski, S., Tanenbaum, D. M., White, T. J., Sninsky, J. J., Hernandez, R. D., Civello, D., Adams, M. D., Cargill, M., and Clark, A. G. (2005). Natural selection on protein‐coding genes in the human genome. Nature 437: 1153–1157.Find this resource:

Buxhoeveden, D. P. and Casanova, M. (2000). Comparative lateralisation patterns in the language area of human, chimpanzee, and rhesus monkey brains. Laterality 4: 315–330.Find this resource:

Buxhoeveden, D. P., Switala, A. E., Litaker, M., Roy, E., and Casanova, M. F. (2001a). Lateralization of minicolumns in human planum temporale is absent in non‐human primate cortex. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 57: 349–358.Find this resource:

Buxhoeveden, D. P., Switala, A. E., Litaker, M., Roy, E., and Casanova, M. F. (2001b). Morphological differences between minicolumns in human and nonhuman primate cortex. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 115: 361–371.Find this resource:

Bybee, J. L. (1998). A functionalist approach to grammar and its evolution. Evolution of Communication 2: 249–278.Find this resource:

Bybee, J. L. (2006a). Frequency of use and the organization of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (p. 649) Find this resource:

Bybee, J. L. (2006b). From usage to grammar: The mind's response to repetition. Language 82: 711–733.Find this resource:

Bybee, J. L. (2009). Language universals and usage‐based theory. In M. H. Christiansen, C. Collins, and S. Edelman (eds.), Language universals. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 17–39.Find this resource:

Bybee, J. L. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Bybee, J. L. and McClelland, J. (2005). Alternatives to the combinatorial paradigm of linguistic theory based on domain general principles of human cognition. The Linguistic Review 22: 381–410.Find this resource:

Bybee, J. L., Perkins, R., and Pagliuca, W. (1994). The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W. (1995). The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W. and Bates, L. A. (2010). Primate social cognition: Uniquely primate, uniquely social, or just unique? Neuron 65: 815–830.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W. and Byrne, J. M. E. (1991). Hand preferences in the skilled gathering tasks of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei). Cortex: 27: 521–46.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W. and Byrne, J. M. E. (1993). Complex leaf‐gathering skills of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei): Variability and standardization. American Journal of Primatology 31: 241–261.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W. and Corp, N. (2004). Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 271: 1693–1699.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W. and Russon, A. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 667–721.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W. and Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W. and Whiten, A. (1992). Cognitive evolution in primates: Evidence from tactical deception. Man NS 27: 609–627.Find this resource:

Byrne, R. W., Bates, L.A., and Moss, C. J. (2009). Elephant cognition in primate perspective. Comparative cognition and behavior reviews 4: 65–79.Find this resource:

Cain, D. P. and Wada, J. A. (1979). An anatomical asymmetry in the baboon brain. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 16: 222–226.Find this resource:

Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (eds.) (2007). The gestural communication of apes and monkeys. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum/London: Taylor and Francis.Find this resource:

Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Science 12: 187–192.Find this resource:

Calvin, W. H. (1985). A stone's throw and its launch window: Timing, precision and its implications for language and hominid brains. Journal of Theoretical Biology 104: 121–135.Find this resource:

Calvin, W. H. (1993). The unitary hypothesis: A common neural circuitry for novel manipulations, language, plan‐ahead and throwing? In Gibson and Ingold (eds.), 230–250.Find this resource:

Calvin, W. H. and Bickerton, D. (2000). Lingua ex machina: Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the human brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Campbell, E. J. M. (1968). The respiratory muscles. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 155: 135–40.Find this resource:

Campbell, L. (2010). Language isolates and their history: What's weird anyway? Paper presented at 36th Annual Meeting, Berkeley Linguistics Society. (p. 650) Find this resource:

Campbell, L. and Poser, W. J. (2008). Language classification: History and method. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Camps, M. and Uriagereka, J. (2006). The Gordian knot of linguistic fossils. In J. Rossello and J. Martin (eds.), The biolinguistic turn. Barcelona: Publicacions Universitat de Barcelona, 34–65.Find this resource:

Cangelosi, A. (2001). Evolution of communication and language using signals, symbols and words. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation 5: 93–101.Find this resource:

Cangelosi, A. (2010). Grounding language in action and perception: From cognitive agents to humanoid robots. Physics of Life Reviews 7: 139–151.Find this resource:

Cangelosi, A. and Harnad, S. (2000). The adaptive advantage of symbolic theft over sensorimotor toil: Grounding language in perceptual categories. Evolution of Communication 4: 117–142.Find this resource:

Cangelosi, A. and Parisi, D. (1998). The emergence of a ‘language’ in an evolving population of neural networks. Connection Science 10: 83–97.Find this resource:

Cangelosi, A. and Parisi, D. (eds.) (2002). Simulating the evolution of language. Berlin: Springer Verlag.Find this resource:

Cangelosi, A., Smith, A. D. M., and Smith, K. (eds.) (2006). Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on the Evolution of Language (EVOLANG6). Singapore: World Scientific.Find this resource:

Cann, R. L., Stoneking, M., and Wilson, A. C. (1987). Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution. Nature 325: 31–36.Find this resource:

Cantalupo, C. and Hopkins, W. D. (2001). Asymmetric Broca's area in Great Apes. Nature 414: 505.Find this resource:

Capasso, L., Michetti, E., and D'Anastasio, R. (2008). A Homo erectus hyoid bone: Possible implications for the origin of the human capability for speech. Collegium Antropologicum 4: 1007–1011.Find this resource:

Carlson, S. M. and Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development 72: 1032–1053.Find this resource:

Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., and Claxton, L. J. (2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 87: 299–319.Find this resource:

Carroll, S. B. (2003). Genetics and the making of Homo sapiens. Nature 422: 849–857.Find this resource:

Carroll, S. B. (2005). Endless forms most beautiful: The new science of evo devo and the making of the animal kingdom. New York: W. W. Norton.Find this resource:

Carroll, S. B. and Boyd, C. (1992). Host race radiation in the soapberry bug: Natural history with the history. Evolution 46: 1052–1069.Find this resource:

Carroll, S. B., Dingle, H., and Klassen, S. P. (1997). Genetic differentiation of fitness‐associated traits among rapidly evolving populations of the soapberry bug. Evolution 51: 1182–1188.Find this resource:

Carroll, S. B., Loye, J. E., Dingle, H., Mathieson, M., Famula, T. R., and Zalucki, M. P. (2005). And the beak shall inherit – evolution in response to invasion. Ecology Letters 8: 944–951.Find this resource:

Carroll, S. B., Hendry, A. P., Reznick, D. N., and Fox, C. W. (2007). Evolution on ecological time‐scales. Functional Ecology 21: 387–393.Find this resource:

Carruthers, P. (2006). The architecture of the mind: Massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (p. 651) Find this resource:

Carstairs‐McCarthy, A. (1999). The origins of complex language: An inquiry into the evolutionary beginnings of sentence, syllables and truth. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Carstairs‐McCarthy, A. (2005). The evolutionary origin of morphology. In Tallerman (ed.), 166–184.Find this resource:

Carstairs‐McCarthy, A. (2010). The evolution of morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Cartmill, E. A. and Byrne, R. W. (2007). Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their audience's comprehension. Current Biology 17: 1345–1348.Find this resource:

Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood (Developmental psychology series). New York: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Catani, M. and ffytche, D. H. (2005). The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes. Brain 128: 2224–2239.Find this resource:

Catchpole, C. K. (2000). Sexual selection and the evolution of song and brain structure in Acrocephalus warblers. In P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, C. T. Snowdon, and T. J. Roper (eds.), Advances in the study of behavior 29. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 45–97.Find this resource:

Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. J. B. (1995/2008), Bird song: Biological themes and variations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Cavalli‐Sforza, L. L. and Feldman, M. W. (2003). The application of molecular approaches to the study of human evolution. Nature Genetics 33 (Suppl): 266–275.Find this resource:

Chamberlain, A. T. and Wood, B. A. (1985). A reappraisal of variation in hominid mandibular corpus dimensions. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 66: 399–405.Find this resource:

Changeux, J. P. and Chavallion, J. (eds.) (1995). Origins of the human brain. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Find this resource:

Chase, P. G. (1999). Symbolism as reference and symbolism as culture. In Dunbar et al. (eds.), 34–49.Find this resource:

Chater, N. and Christiansen, M. H. (2007). Two views of simplicity in linguistic theory: Which connects better with cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 324–326.Find this resource:

Chater, N. and Christiansen, M. H. (2010). Language acquisition meets language evolution. Cognitive Science 34: 1131–1157.Find this resource:

Chater, N. and Vitányi, P. (2007). ‘Ideal learning’ of natural language: Positive results about learning from positive evidence. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 51: 135–163.Find this resource:

Chater, N., Reali, F., and Christiansen, M. H. (2009). Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 106: 1015–1020.Find this resource:

Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1998). Why monkeys don't have language. In G. Petersen (ed.), The Tanner lectures on human values, Vol. 19. Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press, 175–219.Find this resource:

Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (2005). Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. The Linguistic Review 22: 135–159.Find this resource:

Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Cheng, L. L. S. (1997). On the typology of wh‐questions. New York: Garland.Find this resource:

Chicoisne, G., Blondin‐Massé, A., Picard, O., and Harnad, S. (2008). Grounding abstract word definitions in prior concrete experience. Paper presented at the Sixth (p. 652) Annual Conference on the Mental Lexicon, University of Alberta, Banff Alberta, 7–10 October 2008. http://eprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/16618/

Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of ‘Verbal behavior’ by B. F. Skinner. Language 35: 26–58.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1966). Cartesian linguistics. New York: Harper and Row.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. New York: Pantheon.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language. New York: Praeger.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (1999). Reluctant icon: Noam Chomsky interviewed by Tim Halle. http://www.chomsky.info/interviews/1999‐‐‐‐.htm. Accessed 1 July 2008.

Chomsky, N. (2000a). On nature and language. New York and Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (2000b). The architecture of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivation by phase. In M. Kenstowicz (ed.), Ken Hale. A life in language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1–52.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (2005). Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 1–22.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (2007). Approaching UG from below. In U. Sauerland and H. M. Gärtner (eds.), Interfaces + recursion = language? Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 1–29.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (2008). On phases. In R. Freidin, C. Otero, and M.‐L. Zubizarreta (eds.), Foundational issues in linguistic theory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 133–166.Find this resource:

Chomsky, N. (2010). Some simple evo devo theses: How true might they be for language? In Larson et al. (eds.), 45–62.Find this resource:

Chou, H. H., Hayakawa, T., Diaz, S., Krings, M., Indriati, E., Leakey, M., Pääbo, S., Satta, Y., Takahata, N., and Varki, A. (2002). Inactivation of CMP‐N‐acetylneuraminic acid hydroxylase occurred prior to brain expansion during human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 99: 11736–11741.Find this resource:

Christiansen, M. H. and Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31: 489–509.Find this resource:

Christiansen, M. H. and Dale, R. (2004). The role of learning and development in the evolution of language: A connectionist perspective. In Oller and Griebel (eds.), 90–109.Find this resource:

Christiansen, M. H. and Devlin, J. T. (1997). Recursive inconsistencies are hard to learn: A connectionist perspective on universal word order correlations. In M. G. Shafto and P. Langley (eds.), Proceedings of the 19th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 113–118.Find this resource:

Christiansen, M. H. and Kirby, S. (2003). Language evolution: The hardest problem in science? In Christiansen and Kirby (eds.), 1–15.Find this resource:

Christiansen, M. H. and Kirby, S. (eds.) (2003). Language evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Christiansen, M. H., Kelly, L., Shillcock, R., and Greenfield, K. (2008). Impaired artificial grammar learning in agrammatism. Submitted manuscript.Find this resource:

Church, R. B. and Goldin‐Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition 23: 43–71. (p. 653) Find this resource:

Churchill, S. E., Francisco, R. G., McKean‐Peraza, H. A., Daniel, J. A., and Waren B. R. (2009). Shanidar 3 Neandertal rib puncture wound and Paleolithic weaponry. Journal of Human Evolution 57: 163–178.Find this resource:

Clapham, P. J., Leimkuhler, E., Gray, B. K., and Mattila, D. K. (1995). Do humpback whales exhibit lateralized behaviour? Animal Behaviour 50: 73–82.Find this resource:

Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Clark, C. W. (1995). Application of US Navy underwater hydrophone arrays for scientific research on whales. Reports of the International Whaling Commission 45: 210–212.Find this resource:

Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Clarke, E., Reichard, U., and Zuberbühler, K. (2006). The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS ONE 1: e73.Find this resource:

Clay, Z. and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Food‐associated calling sequences in bonobos. Animal Behaviour 77: 1387–1396.Find this resource:

Clay, Z. and Zuberbühler, K. (2011). Bonobos extract meaning from call sequences. PLoS ONE 6: e18786.Find this resource:

Clayton, N. S. and Dickinson, A. (1998). Episodic‐like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395: 272–274.Find this resource:

Clayton, N. S., Griffiths, D., Emery, N., and Dickinson, A. (2001). Elements of episodic‐like memory in animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 356: 1483–1491.Find this resource:

Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J., Emery, N. J., and Dickinson, A. (2003). Prometheus to Proust: The case for behavioural criteria for mental ‘time travel’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 436–437.Find this resource:

Clutton‐Brock, T. H. (2006). Cooperative breeding in mammals. In P. M. Kappeler and C. P. Van Schaik (eds.), Cooperation in primates and humans. Berlin: Springer Verlag, 173–190.Find this resource:

Cochran, G. and Harpending, H. (2009). The 10,000 year explosion: How civilization accelerated human evolution. New York: Basic Books.Find this resource:

Cohen, H. and Lefebvre, C. (eds.) (2005). Handbook of categorization in cognitive science. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Find this resource:

Comrie, B. (1989). Language universals and linguistic typology: Syntax and morphology (2nd edn.). Oxford: Blackwell.Find this resource:

Comrie, B. (1992). Before complexity. In Hawkins and Gell‐Mann (eds.), 193–211.Find this resource:

Comrie, B. (2000). From potential to realization: An episode in the origin of language. Linguistics 38: 989–1004.Find this resource:

Comrie, B. (2003). Reconstruction, typology and reality. In Hickey (ed.), 243–257.Find this resource:

Connor, R. C. and Smolker, R. A. (1996). ‘Pop’ goes the dolphin: A vocalization male bottlenose dolphins produce during consortships. Behaviour 133: 643–662.Find this resource:

Converse, L. J., Carlson, A. A., Ziegler, T. E., and Snowdon, C. T. (1995). Communication of ovulatory state to mates by female pygmy marmosets, Cebuella pygmaea. Animal Behaviour 49: 615–621.Find this resource:

Conway, C. M. and Christiansen, M. H. (2001). Sequential learning in non‐human primates. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5: 539–546.Find this resource:

Cook, M. L. H., Sayigh, L. S., Blum, J. E., and Wells, R. S. (2004). Signature‐whistle production in undisturbed free‐ranging bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 271: 1043–1049. (p. 654) Find this resource:

Cook, R., Brown, M., and Riley, D. (1983). Flexible memory processing by rats: Use of prospective and retrospective information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 11: 453–469.Find this resource:

Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., and Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2007). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition 106: 1047–1058.Find this resource:

Coolidge, F. L. and Wynn, T. (2001). Executive functions of the frontal lobes and the evolutionary ascendancy of Homo sapiens. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 11: 255–260.Find this resource:

Coolidge, F. L. and Wynn, T. (2005). Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 15: 5–26.Find this resource:

Coolidge, F. L. and Wynn, T. (2009a). Recursion, phonological storage capacity, and the evolution of modern speech. In Botha and Knight (eds.) (2009b), 244–254.Find this resource:

Coolidge, F. L. and Wynn, T. (2009b). The rise of Homo sapiens: The evolution of modern thinking. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.Find this resource:

Cooper, D. L. (2006). Broca's Arrow: Evolution, prediction and language in the brain. The Anatomical Record (Part B: New Anatomy) 289B: 9–24.Find this resource:

Cooper, M. D. and Alder, M. N. (2006). The evolution of adaptive immune systems. Cell 124: 815–822.Find this resource:

Corballis, M. C. (1993). The lopsided ape: Evolution of the generative mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Find this resource:

Corballis, M. C. (2003). From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right‐handedness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26: 199–260.Find this resource:

Corballis, M. C. (2004). The origins of modernity: Was autonomous speech the critical factor? Psychological Review 111: 543–552.Find this resource:

Corballis, M. C. (2009). Mental time travel and the shaping of language. Experimental Brain Research 192: 553–560.Find this resource:

Corballis, M. C. and Lea, S. E. G. (eds.) (1999). The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Coudé, G., Ferrari, P. F., Roda, F., and Maranesi, M., Veroni, V., Monti, F., Rizzolatti, G., and Fogassi, L. (2007). Neuronal responses during vocalization in the ventral premotor cortex of macaque monkeys. Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting (San Diego California), Abstract 636.3.Find this resource:

Crain, S. (1991). Language acquisition in absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14: 597–650.Find this resource:

Crockford, C. and Boesch, C. (2003). Context‐specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: Analysis of barks. Animal Behaviour 66: 115–125.Find this resource:

Crockford, C. and Boesch, C. (2005). Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 142: 397–421.Find this resource:

Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L., and Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group‐specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology 110: 221–243.Find this resource:

Croft, W. A. (1991). The evolution of negation. Journal of Linguistics 27: 1–27.Find this resource:

Croft, W. A. (2000). Explaining language change. Harlow: Longman Linguistic Library.Find this resource:

Croft, W. A. (2001). Radical construction grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (p. 655) Find this resource:

Crow, T. J. (2004). Directional asymmetry is the key to the origin of modern Homo sapiens (the Broca‐Annett axiom): A reply to Rogers' review of ‘The speciation of modern Homo sapiens’. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition 9: 233–242.Find this resource:

Culicover, P. W. and Jackendoff, R. (2005). Simpler syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Currat, M., Excoffier, L., Maddison, W, Otto, S. P., Ray, N., Whitlock, M. C., and Yeaman, S. (2006). Comment on ‘Ongoing Adaptive Evolution of ASPM, a Brain Size Determinant in Homo sapiens’ and ‘Microcephalin, a Gene Regulating Brain Size, Continues to Evolve Adaptively in Humans’. Science 313: 172.Find this resource:

Curtiss, S. (1977). Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a modern‐day ‘wild child’. New York: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Dahl, Ö. (2004). The growth and maintenance of linguistic complexity. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Find this resource:

Damasio, H. and Damasio, A. R. (1989). Lesion analysis in neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Dart, R. A. (1925). Australopithecus africanus: The man‐ape of South Africa. Nature 115: 195–199.Find this resource:

Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.Find this resource:

Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.Find this resource:

Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.) (2002). Imitation in animals and artifacts: Complex adaptive systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Davidson, I. (2003). The archaeological evidence of language origins: State of art. In Christiansen and Kirby (eds.), 140–157.Find this resource:

Davidson, R. J., Shackman, A. J., and Maxwell, J. S. (2004). Asymmetries in face and brain related to emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 389–391.Find this resource:

Davies, A. G. and Oates, J. F. (1994). Colobine monkeys: Their ecology, behaviour and evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Dawkins, R. (1986). The blind watchmaker. New York: W. W. Norton.Find this resource:

Dawkins, R. (1995). River out of Eden: A Darwinian view of life. New York: Basic Books.Find this resource:

Dawkins, R. (1996). Climbing mount improbable. New York: W. W. Norton.Find this resource:

Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: The coevolution of language and the brain. New York: W. W. Norton and Company/London: Penguin Press.Find this resource:

Deacon, T. W. (2003). Universal Grammar and semiotic constraints. In Christiansen and Kirby (eds.), 111–140.Find this resource:

Deacon, T. W. (2009). Relaxed selection and the role of epigenesis in the evolution of language. In M. S. Blumberg, J. H. Freeman, and S. R. Robinson (eds.), Oxford handbook of developmental behavioral neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press, 730–752.Find this resource:

Deacon, T. W. (2010). A role for relaxed selection in the evolution of the language capacity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 107: 9000–9006.Find this resource:

de Boer, B. (2000). Self organization in vowel systems. Journal of Phonetics 28: 441–465.Find this resource:

de Boer, B. (2001). The origins of vowel systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

de Boer, B. (2003). Conditions for stable vowel systems in a population. In W. Banzhaf, T. Christaller, P. Dittrich, J. T. Kim, and J. Ziegler (eds.), Advances in artificial life: Lecture notes in computer science 2801. Berlin: Springer, 415–424.Find this resource:

de Boer, B. (2005a). Infant directed speech and the evolution of language. In Tallerman (ed.), 100–121. (p. 656) Find this resource:

de Boer, B. (2005b). Self‐organisation in language. In C. Hemelrijk (ed.), Self‐organisation and evolution of social systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 123–139.Find this resource:

de Boer, B. and Kuhl, P. K. (2003). Investigating the role of infant‐directed speech with a computer model. Acoustics Research Letters Online 4: 129–134.Find this resource:

de Castro, J. and Nicolas, M. (1997). Palaeodemography of the Atapuerca‐SH Middle Pleistocene hominid sample. Journal of Human Evolution 33: 333–355.Find this resource:

Dediu, D. and Ladd, D. R. (2007). Linguistic tone is related to the population frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two brain size genes: ASPM and Microcephalin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104: 10944–10949.Find this resource:

DeGusta, D., Gilbert, W. H., and Turner, S. P. (1999). Hypoglossal canal size and hominid speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 96: 1800–1804.Find this resource:

Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A., Dehaene‐Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., and Cohen, L. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330: 1359–1364.Find this resource:

de la Torre, S. and Snowdon, C. T. (2009). Dialects in pygmy marmosets? Population variation in call structure. American Journal of Primatology 71: 333–342.Find this resource:

de la Torre, I., Mora, R., Dominguez‐Rodrigo, M., de Luque, L., and Alcala, L. (2003). The Oldowan industry of Peninj and its bearing on the reconstruction of the technological skill of Lower Pleistocene hominids. Journal of Human Evolution 44: 203–224.Find this resource:

Delgado, R. A. (2006). Sexual selection in the loud calls of male primates: Signal content and function. International Journal of Primatology 27: 5–25.Find this resource:

Dennell, R. and Roebroeks, W. (2005). An Asian perspective on early human dispersal from Africa. Nature 438: 1099–1104.Find this resource:

Dennett, D. (1995). Darwin's dangerous idea: Evolution and the meaning of life. New York: Simon and Schuster.Find this resource:

Dent, M. L., Pierce, A., Brittan‐Powell, E. F., and Dooling, R. J. (1997). Perception of synthetic /ba/‐/wa/ speech continuum by budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulates). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 102: 1891–1897.Find this resource:

d'Errico, F. (2005). The invisible frontier: A multiple species model for the origin of behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 188–202.Find this resource:

d'Errico, F. and Vanhaeren, M. (2009). Earliest personal ornaments and their significance for the origin of language debate. In Botha and Knight (eds.) (2009a), 16–40.Find this resource:

d'Errico F., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., Tillier, A.‐M., Soressi, M., Bresson, F., Maureille, B., Nowell, A., Lakara, J., Backwell, L., and Julien, M. (2003). Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music – An alternative multidisciplinary perspective. Journal of World Prehistory 17: 1–70.Find this resource:

d'Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Vanhaeren, M., and Van Niekerk, K. (2005). Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: Evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution 48: 3–24.Find this resource:

d'Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., Barton, N., Bouzouggar, A., Mienis, H., Richter, D., Hublin, J‐J., McPherron, S., and Lozouet, P. (2009). Additional evidence on the use of personal ornaments in the Middle Paleolithic of North Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 106: 16051–16056. (p. 657) Find this resource:

De Smet, H. J., Baillieus, H., De Deyn, P. P., Marien, P., and Paquier, P. (2007). The cerebellum and language: The story so far. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica 7: 165–170.Find this resource:

de Sousa, A. and Wood, B. (2007). The hominin fossil record and the emergence of the modern human central nervous system. In J. H. Kaas (ed.), Evolution of nervous systems: A comprehensive reference. Vol. 4: The evolution of primate nervous systems. Oxford: Elsevier, 291–336.Find this resource:

Dessalles, J.‐L. (1998). Altruism, status and the origin of relevance. In Hurford et al. (eds.), 130–147.Find this resource:

De Vylder, B. and Tuyls, K. (2006). How to reach linguistic consensus: A proof of convergence for the naming game. Journal of Theoretical Biology 242: 818–831.Find this resource:

de Waal, F. B. M. (1982). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. London: Jonathan Cape/New York: Harper and Row.Find this resource:

de Waal, F. B. M. (1988). The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus) compared to that of chimpanzees. Behaviour 106: 183–251.Find this resource:

de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Darwin's legacy and the study of primate visual communication. In P. Ekman, J. J. Campos, R. J. Davidson, and F. B. M. de Waal (eds.), Emotions inside out: 130 years after Darwin's ‘The expression of the emotions in man and animals’. New York: New York Academy of Sciences, 7–31.Find this resource:

de Waal, F. B. M. and van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1981). Side‐directed communication and agonistic interactions in chimpanzees. Behaviour 77: 164–198.Find this resource:

Diamond, J. (1991/2004). The third chimpanzee: The evolution and future of the human animal. New York: Harper Collins.Find this resource:

Diller, K. and Cann, R. L. (2009). Evidence against a genetic‐based revolution in language 50,000 years ago. In Botha and Knight (eds.) (2009a), 135–149.Find this resource:

Di Paolo, E. A., Noble, J., and Bullock, S. (2000). Simulation models as opaque thought experiments. In M. A. Bedau, J. S. McCaskill, N. H. Packard, and S. Rasmussen (eds.), Artificial Life VII: Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference on Artificial Life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 497–506.Find this resource:

Dobzhansky, T. (1937). Genetics and ‘The origin of species’. New York: Columbia University Press.Find this resource:

Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Nothing makes sense in biology except in the light of evolution. The American Biology Teacher 35: 125–129.Find this resource:

Dolgix, B. O. (1960). Rodovoy i plemennoy sostav narodov Sibiri v XVII veke. [The tribal composition of the peoples of Siberia in the 17th century]. Moscow: Izdatel'stvo Akademii Nauk SSSR.Find this resource:

Dominguez‐Rodrigo, M., Serrallonga, J., Juan‐Tresserras, J., Alcala, L., and Luque, L. (2001). Woodworking activities by early humans: A plant residue analysis on Acheulean stone tools from Peninj (Tanzania). Journal of Human Evolution 40: 289–299.Find this resource:

Donald M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Donald, M. (1993). Précis of ‘Origins of the modern mind’ with multiple reviews and author's response. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 737–791.Find this resource:

Donald, M. (1998). Mimesis and the executive suite: Missing links in language evolution. In Hurford et al. (eds.), 44–67.Find this resource:

Donald, M. (1999). Preconditions for the evolution of protolanguages. In Corballis and Lea (eds.), 138–154. (p. 658) Find this resource:

Donald, M. (2009). Sixteen years after the fact: Revisiting a theory of human cognitive evolution. In G. Hatfield and H. Pittman (eds.), The co‐evolution of mind, brain and culture. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Museum of Archaeology and Anthropology.Find this resource:

Dorus, S., Vallender, E. J., Evans, P. D., Mahowald, M., Wyckoff, G. J., Malcom, C. M., and Lahn, B. T. (2004). Accelerated evolution of nervous system genes in the origin of Homo sapiens. Cell 119: 1027–1040.Find this resource:

Doupe, A. J. and Kuhl, P. K. (1999). Birdsong and human speech: Common themes and mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience 22: 567–631.Find this resource:

Drea, C. M. and Carter, A. (2009). Cooperative problem solving in a social carnivore. Animal Behaviour 78: 967–977.Find this resource:

Duchin, L. E. (1990). The evolution of articulate speech: Comparative anatomy of the oral cavity in Pan and Homo. Journal of Human Evolution 19: 687–697.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1991). Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatologica 57: 121–131.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution 22: 469–493.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortex size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 681–735.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber and Faber/Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology 32: 163–181.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Mind the gap: Or why humans aren't just great apes. Proceedings of the British Academy 154: 403–423.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2009). Why only humans have language. In Botha and Knight (eds.) (2009b), 12–35.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. and Dunbar, P. (1975). Social dynamics of gelada baboons. Basel: Karger.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. and Shultz, S. (2007a). Evolution in the social brain. Science 317: 1344–1347.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M. and Shultz, S. (2007b). Understanding primate brain evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 362: 649–658.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M., Duncan, N., and Nettle, D. (1995). Size and structure of freely forming conversational groups. Human Nature 6: 67–78.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M., Duncan, N., and Marriot, A. (1997). Human conversational behaviour. Human Nature 8: 231–246.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M., Knight, C., and Power, C. (eds.) (1999). The evolution of culture: An interdisciplinary view. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Find this resource:

Dunbar, R. I. M., Korstjens, A. H., and Lehmann, J. (2009). Time as an ecological constraint. Biological Reviews 84: 413–429.Find this resource:

Durie, M. and Ross, M. (eds.) (1996). The comparative method reviewed: Regularity and irregularity in language change. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Edelman, G. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books.Find this resource:

Egnor, S. E. R. and Hauser, M. D. (2004). A paradox in the evolution of primate vocal learning. Trends in Neurosciences 27: 649–654.Find this resource:

Ekman, P. (ed.) (1972a). Emotion in the human face: Guide‐lines for research and an integration of findings. New York: Pergamon Press. (p. 659) Find this resource:

Ekman, P. (1972b). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In J. Cole (ed.), Nebraska Symposium on motivation 1971. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press, 207–283.Find this resource:

Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of non‐verbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica 1: 49–98.Find this resource:

Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 17: 124–129.Find this resource:

Ellis, N. C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18: 91–126.Find this resource:

Ellis, N. C. and Larsen‐Freeman, D. (eds.) (2009). Language learning Volume 59: Supplement 1.Find this resource:

Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science 14: 179–211.Find this resource:

Elman, J. L., Bates, E., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff‐Smith, A., Parisi, D., and Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Elowson, A. M. and Snowdon, C. T. (1994). Pygmy marmosets, Cebuella pygmaea, modify vocal structure in response to changed social environment. Animal Behaviour 47: 1267–1277.Find this resource:

Elowson, A. M., Snowdon, C. T., and Lazaro‐Perea, C. (1998a). ‘Babbling’ and social context in infant monkeys: Parallels to human infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2: 31–37.Find this resource:

Elowson, A. M., Snowdon, C. T., and Lazaro‐Perea, C. (1998b). Infant ‘babbling’ in a nonhuman primate: Complex vocal sequences with repeated call types. Behaviour 135: 643–664.Find this resource:

Emery, N. J. and Clayton, N. S. (2001). Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414: 443–446.Find this resource:

Emery, N. J. and Clayton, N. S. (2004). The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science 306: 1903–1907.Find this resource:

Emmorey, K. (1999). Do signers gesture? In L. S. Messing and R. Campbell (eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 133–159.Find this resource:

Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition, and brain: Insights from sign language research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Find this resource:

Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., Monaco, A., and Pääbo, S. (2002). Molecular evolution of FOXP2: A gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418: 869–872.Find this resource:

Endicott, P., Ho, S., and Stringer, C. B. (2010). Using genetic evidence to evaluate four palaeoanthropological hypotheses for the timing of Neanderthal and modern human origins. Journal of Human Evolution 59: 87–95.Find this resource:

Enfield, N. J. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.) (2006). Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction. Oxford: Berg.Find this resource:

Engh, A. E., Hoffmeier, R., Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (2006). Who, me? Can baboons infer the target of a vocalization? Animal Behaviour. 71: 381–387.Find this resource:

Engle, R. W. and Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two‐factor theory of cognitive control. In B. Ross (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 44. New York, NY: Elsevier, 145–199.Find this resource:

Erdal, D. and Whiten, A. (1994). On human egalitarianism: An evolutionary product of Machiavellian status escalation? Current Anthropology 35: 175–183. (p. 660) Find this resource:

Eriksson, P. S., Perfilieva, E., Björk‐Eriksson, T., Alborn, A., Nordborg, C., Peterson, D. A., and Gage, F. H. (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nature Medicine 4: 1313–1317.Find this resource:

Erman, B. and Warren, B. (2000). The idiom principle and the open choice principle. Text 20: 29–62.Find this resource:

Erren, T. C., Cullen, P., and Erren, M. (2008). Neanderthal, chimp and human genomes: Hypotheses wanted for research into brain evolution. Medical Hypotheses 70: 4–7.Find this resource:

Eswaran, V., Harpending, H., and Rogers, A. R. (2005). Genomics refutes an exclusively African origin of humans. Journal of Human Evolution 49: 1–18.Find this resource:

Ettlinger, G. F. (1988). Hand preference, ability and hemispheric specialization. How far are these factors related in the monkey? Cortex 24: 389–398.Find this resource:

Evans, C. S., Evans, L., and Marler, P. (1993). On the meaning of alarm calls: Functional reference in an avian vocal system. Animal Behaviour 46: 23–38.Find this resource:

Evans, N. (ed.) (2003). The Non‐Pama‐Nyungan languages of Northern Australia: Comparative studies of the continent's most linguistically complex region. Canberra: Research School of Pacific Studies, Australian National University.Find this resource:

Evans, N. and Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32: 429–492.Find this resource:

Evans, P. D., Anderson, J. R., Vallender, E. J., Choi, S. S., and Lahn, B. T. (2004a). Reconstructing the evolutionary history of microcephalin, a gene controlling human brain size. Human Molecular Genetics, 13: 1139–1145.Find this resource:

Evans, P. D., Anderson, J. R., Vallender, E. J., Gilbert, S. L., Malcom, C. M., Dorus, S., and Lahn, B. T. (2004b). Adaptive evolution of ASPM, a major determinant of cerebral cortical size in humans. Human Molecular Genetics 13: 489–494.Find this resource:

Evans, P. D., Gilbert, S. L., Mekel‐Bobrov, N., Vallender, E. J., Anderson, J. R., Vaez‐Azizi, L. M., Tishkoff, S. A., Hudson, R. R., and Lahn, B. T. (2005). Microcephalin, a gene regulating brain size, continues to evolve adaptively in humans. Science 309: 1717–1720.Find this resource:

Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology 46: 621–646.Find this resource:

Everett, D. L. (2009). Pirahã culture and grammar: a response to some criticisms. Language 85: 405–442.Find this resource:

Fagot, J. and Vauclair, J. (1993). Manual and hemispheric specialization in the manipulation of a joystick by baboons (Papio papio). Behavioral Neuroscience 107: 210–214.Find this resource:

Falk, D. (1980). A re‐analysis of the South African Australopithecine natural endocasts. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 53: 525–539.Find this resource:

Falk, D. (1983). The Taung endocast: A reply to Holloway. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 60: 479–480.Find this resource:

Falk, D. (2004). Prelinguistic evolution in early Hominins: Whence motherese? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 491–503.Find this resource:

Falk, D. (2009). Finding our tongues: Mothers, infants and the origins of language. New York: Basic Books.Find this resource:

Falk, D. and Gibson, K. R. (eds.) (2001). External anatomy of the primate cerebral cortex. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Falk, D., Cheverud, J., Vannier, M. W., and Conroy, G. C. (1986). Advanced computer‐graphics technology reveals cortical asymmetry in endocasts of rhesus monkeys. Folia Primatologica 46: 98–103. (p. 661) Find this resource:

Falk, D., Hildebolt, C., Cheverud, J., Vannier, M., Helmkamp, R. C., and Konigsberg, L. (1990). Cortical asymmetries in the frontal lobe of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Brain Research 512: 40–45.Find this resource:

Fant, G. (1960). Acoustic theory of sound production. The Hague: Mouton.Find this resource:

Farrar, M. J. (1990). Discourse and the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 17: 607–624.Find this resource:

Farries, M. A. (2004). The avian song system in comparative perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1016: 61–76.Find this resource:

Feenders, G., Liedvogel, M., Rivas, J., Zapka, M., Horita, H., Hara, E., Wada, K., Mouritsen, H., and Jarvis, E. D. (2008). Molecular mapping of movement‐associated areas in the avian brain: A motor theory for vocal learning. PLoS ONE 3: e1768.Find this resource:

Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist 66: 103–114.Find this resource:

Ferguson, C. A. and Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language 51: 419–430.Find this resource:

Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month‐old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development 8: 181–195.Find this resource:

Fernald, A. (1992). Meaningful melodies in mothers' speech. In H. Papoušek, U. Jürgens, and M. Papoušek (eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 262–282.Find this resource:

Fernald, A. (1994). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In P. Bloom (ed.), Language acquisition: Core readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 51–94.Find this resource:

Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., de Boysson‐Bardies, B., and Fukui, I. (1989). A cross‐language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language 16: 477–501.Find this resource:

Fernald, R. D. (2003). How does behavior change the brain? Multiple methods to answer old questions. Integrative and Comparative Biology 43: 771–779.Find this resource:

Fernandez‐Carriba, S., Loeches, A., Morcillo, A., and Hopkins, W. D. (2002). Asymmetry in facial expression of emotions by chimpanzees. Neuropsychologia 40: 1523–1533.Find this resource:

Ferrari, P. F., Gallese, P., Rizzolatti, G., and Fogassi, L. (2003). Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth movements in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience 17: 1703–1714.Find this resource:

Fillmore, C. J., Kay, P., and O'Connor, M. C. (1988). Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions. Language 64: 501–538.Find this resource:

Finlay, B. L. (2007). Endless minds most beautiful. Developmental Science 10: 30–34.Find this resource:

Finlay, B. L., and Darlington, R. B. (1995). Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science 268: 1578–1584.Find this resource:

Finlayson, C. (2004). Neanderthals and modern humans: An ecological and evolutionary perspective. New York and Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Fisher, C. and Gleitman, L. R. (2002). Language acquisition. In H. F. Pashler and C. R. Gallistel (eds.), Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology, Vol. 3: Learning and motivation. New York: Wiley, 445–496.Find this resource:

Fisher, R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Find this resource:

Fisman, R., Iyengar, S. S., Kamenica, E., and Simonson, I. (2006). Gender differences in mate selection: Evidence from a speed dating experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics 121: 673–697. (p. 662) Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. (2000a). The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 258–267.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. (2000b). The phonetic potential of nonhuman vocal tracts: Comparative cineradiographic observations of vocalizing animals. Phonetics 57: 205–218.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. (2002). Comparative vocal production and the evolution of speech: Reinterpreting the descent of the larynx. In Wray (ed.), 21–45.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. (2004). Kin selection and ‘mother tongues’: A neglected component in language evolution. In Oller and Griebel (eds.), 275–296.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. (2005). The evolution of language: A comparative review. Biology and Philosophy 20: 193–230.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. (2007). The evolution of language: A comparative perspective. In G. Gaskell (ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 787–804.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. (2010a). The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. (2010b). Three meanings of ‘recursion’: Key distinctions for biolinguistics. In Larson et al. (eds.), 73–90.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. and Hauser, M. D. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303: 377–380.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T. and Reby, D. (2001). The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 268: 1669–1675.Find this resource:

Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., and Chomsky, N. (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition 97: 179–210.Find this resource:

Flavell, J. H. (1992). Perspectives on perspective taking. In H. Beilin and P. Pufall (eds.), Piaget's theory: Prospects and possibilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 107–139.Find this resource:

Fleischman, S. (1982). The future in thought and language: Diachronic evidence from Romance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Focquaert, F., Braeckman, J., and Platek, S. (2008). An evolutionary cognitive neuroscience perspective on human self‐awareness and theory of mind. Philosophical Psychology 21: 47–68.Find this resource:

Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Fodor, J. A. and Piattelli‐Palmarini, M. (2010). What Darwin got wrong. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Find this resource:

Ford, J. K. B. (1991). Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 1454–1483.Find this resource:

Forgacs, G. and Newman, S. (2005). Biological physics of the developing embryo. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Forster, P. and Matsumura, S. (2005). Did early humans go north or south? Science 308: 965–966.Find this resource:

Forster, S. and Cords, M. (2005). Socialization of infant blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni): Allomaternal interactions and sex differences. Behaviour 142: 869–896.Find this resource:

Forsyth, J. (1992). A history of the peoples of Siberia, Russia's North Asian colony 1581–1990. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Foundas, A. L., Leonard, C., and Heilman, K. (1995). Morphological cerebral asymmetries and handedness: The pars triangularis and planum temporale. Archives of Neurology 52: 501–508. (p. 663) Find this resource:

Foundas, A. L., Eure, K. F., Luevano, L. F., and Weinberger, D. R. (1998). MRI asymmetries of Broca's area: The pars triangularis and pars opercularis. Brain and Language 64: 282–296.Find this resource:

Fouts, R. S. (1975). Capacities for language in the great apes. In R. H. Tuttle (ed.), Socioecology and psychology of primates. The Hague/Paris: Mouton, 371–390.Find this resource:

Fouts, R. S. and Mills, S. T. (1997). Next of kin: My conversations with chimpanzees. New York: William Morrow.Find this resource:

Fouts, R. S. and Rigby, R. L. (1977). Man‐chimpanzee communication. In Sebeok (ed.), 1034–1054.Find this resource:

Frankel, A. S., Clark, C. W., Herman, L. M., and Gabriele, C. M. (1995). Spatial distribution, habitat utilization, and social interactions of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, off Hawai'i, determined using acoustic and visual techniques. Canadian Journal of Zoology 73: 1134–1146.Find this resource:

Franks, B. K., and Rigby, K. (2005). Deception and mate selection: Some implications for relevance and the evolution of language. In Tallerman (ed.), 208–229.Find this resource:

Frayer, D. W. and Nicolay, C. (2000). Fossil evidence on the origin of speech sounds. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (eds.), The origin of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 217–234.Find this resource:

Freeberg, T. M. (2006). Social complexity can drive vocal complexity. Psychological Science 17: 557–561.Find this resource:

Fripp, D., Owen, C., Quintana‐Rizzo, E., Shapiro, A., Buckstaff, K., Jankowski, K., Wells, R. S., and Tyack, P. L. (2005). Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) calves appear to model their signature whistles on the signature whistles of community members. Animal Cognition 8: 17–26.Find this resource:

Frishberg, N. (1975). Arbitrariness and iconicity in American Sign Language. Language 51: 696–719.Find this resource:

Fromkin, V. (ed.) (1978). Tone: A linguistic survey. New York: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Fuster, J. M. (1997). The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe (3rd edn.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott‐Raven.Find this resource:

Futuyma, D. J. (1997). Evolutionary biology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Find this resource:

Galantucci, B. (2005). An experimental study of the emergence of human communication systems. Cognitive Science 29: 737–767.Find this resource:

Galantucci, B. and Garrod, S. (2010). Experimental semiotics: A new approach for studying the emergence and the evolution of human communication. Interaction Studies 11: 1–13.Find this resource:

Galantucci, B., Fowler, C. A., and Turvey, M. T. (2006). The motor theory of speech perception reviewed. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 13: 361–377.Find this resource:

Gannon, P. J., Holloway, R. L., Broadfield, D. C., and Braun, A. R. (1998). Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: Humanlike pattern of Wernicke's brain language area homolog. Science 279: 220–222.Find this resource:

Gannon, P. J., Kheck, N. M., Braun, A. R., and Holloway, R. L. (2005). Planum parietale of chimpanzees and orangutans: A comparative resonance of human‐like planum temporale asymmetry. The Anatomical Record 287: 1128–1141.Find this resource:

Gannon, P. J., Kheck, N. M., and Hof, P. R. (2008). Leftward interhemispheric asymmetry of macaque monkey temporal lobe language area homolog is evident at the cytoarchitectural, but not gross anatomic level. Brain Research 1199: 62–73. (p. 664) Find this resource:

Gardner, R. A. and Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 165: 664–672.Find this resource:

Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T., and Van Cantfort, T. E. (eds.) (1989). Teaching sign language to chimpanzees. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Find this resource:

Garstang, W. (1922). The theory of recapitulation: A critical re‐statement of the biogenetic law. Journal of the Linnaean Society (Zoology) 35: 81–101.Find this resource:

Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., and Wearing, H. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology 40: 177–190.Find this resource:

Gazda, S. K., Connor, R. C., Edgar, R. K., and Cox, F. (2005). A division of labour with role specialization in group‐hunting bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off Cedar Key, Florida. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 272: 135–140.Find this resource:

Geissmann, T. (2000). Gibbon songs and human music from an evolutionary perspective. In Wallin et al. (eds.), 103–124.Find this resource:

Gentilucci, M. and Corballis, M. C. (2006). From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 949–960.Find this resource:

Gentner, T. Q., Fenn, K. J., Margoliash, D., and Nusbaum, H. C. (2006). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440: 1204–1207.Find this resource:

Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animal and man. Brain 88: 585–644.Find this resource:

Geschwind, N. and Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations and pathology: I. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of Neurology 42: 428–459.Find this resource:

Geschwind, N. and Levitsky, W. (1968). Human brain: Left‐right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science 161: 186–187.Find this resource:

Ghazanfar, A. and Hauser, M. D. (2001). The auditory behavior of primates: A neuroethological perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11: 712–720.Find this resource:

Ghazanfar, A., Maier, J. X., Hoffman, K. L., and Logothetis, N. (2005). Multisensory integration of dynamic faces and voices in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 25: 5004–5012.Find this resource:

Gibbons, A. (2006). The first human. New York: Doubleday.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. (1990). Neurological perspectives on comparative animal and human intelligence: New approaches to the instinct versus learning controversy. In Parker and Gibson (eds.), 97–128.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. (1991). Myelination and behavioral development: A comparative perspective on questions of neoteny, altriciality, and intelligence. In Gibson and Petersen (eds.), 29–64.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. (1993). Tool use, language and social behavior in relationship to information processing capacities. In Gibson and Ingold (eds.), 251–269.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. (1996a). The ontogeny and evolution of the brain, cognition and language. In Lock and Peters (eds.), 409–433.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. (1996b). The biocultural brain: Seasonal migrations and the emergence of the Upper Paleolithic. In P. A. Mellars and K. R. Gibson (eds.), Modelling the early human mind. McDonald Institute Monographs. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 33–48.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. (2002). Evolution of human intelligence: The roles of brain size and mental construction. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 59: 10–20.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. (2005). Epigenesis, brain plasticity, and behavioral versatility: Alternatives to standard evolution psychology models. In S. McKinnon and S. Silverman (eds.), (p. 665) Complexities: Anthropological challenges to reductive accounts of bio‐social life. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, 23–42.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. and Ingold, T. (eds.) (1993). Tools, language and cognition in human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. and Jessee, S. (1999). Language evolution and expansions of multiple neural processing areas. In B. King (ed.), The evolution of language: Assessing the evidence from the non‐human primates. Santa Fe, NM: School for American Research, 189–228.Find this resource:

Gibson, K. R. and Petersen, A. C. (eds.) (1991). Brain maturation and cognitive development: Comparative and cross‐cultural perspectives. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.Find this resource:

Gil, D. (1994). The structure of Riau Indonesian. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 17: 179–200.Find this resource:

Gil, D. (2000). Syntactic categories, cross‐linguistic variation and universal grammar. In P. M. Vogel and B. Comrie (eds.), Approaches to the typology of word classes. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 173–216.Find this resource:

Gil, D. (2005). Isolating‐Monocategorial‐Associational language. In Cohen and Lefebvre (eds.), 347–379.Find this resource:

Gil, D. (2008). How much grammar does it take to sail a boat? (Or, what can material artefacts tell us about the evolution of language?). In A. D. M. Smith et al. (eds.), 123–130.Find this resource:

Gilbert, W. H. (2008). Hominid systematics. In W. H. Gilbert and B. Asfaw (eds.), Homo erectus. Pleistocene evidence from the Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, 349–371.Find this resource:

Gil‐da‐Costa, R., Braun, A., Lopes, M., Hauser, M. D., Carson, R., Herscovitch, P., and Martin, A. (2004). Toward an evolutionary perspective on conceptual representation: Species‐specific calls activate visual and affective processing systems in the macaque. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 101: 17516–17521.Find this resource:

Gil‐da‐Costa, R., Martin, A., Lopes, M. A., Munoz, M., Fritz, J. B., and Braun, A. R. (2006). Species‐specific calls activate homologs of Broca's and Wernicke's areas in the macaque. Nature Neuroscience 9: 1064–1070.Find this resource:

Gilissen, E. (1992). The neocortical sulci of the capuchin monkey (Cebus): Evidence for asymmetry in the sylvian sulcus and comparison with other primates. Comptes Rendus de l'Academie de Sciences Paris, Series III 314: 165–170.Find this resource:

Gilissen, E. (2001). Structural symmetries and asymmetries in human and chimpanzee brains. In Falk and Gibson (eds.), 187–215.Find this resource:

Giret, N., Miklósi, A., Kreutzer, M., and Bovet, D. (2009). Use of experimenter‐given cues by African Gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Animal Cognition 12: 1–10.Find this resource:

Gisiner, R. C. and Schusterman, R. J. (1992). Combinatorial relationships learned by a language‐trained sea lion. In J. Thomas, R. A. Kastelein, and A. Y. Supin. (eds.), Marine mammal sensory systems. New York: Plenum Press, 643–662.Find this resource:

Givón, T. (1971). Historical syntax and synchronic morphology: An archaeologist's field trip. In D. Adams, M. A. Campbell, V. Cohen, J. Lovins, E. Maxwell, C. Nygren, and J. Reighard (eds.), Papers from the Seventh Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, 394–415.Find this resource:

Givón, T. (1979a). Discourse and syntax. New York: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Givón, T. (1979b). On understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Givón, T. (1998). On the co‐evolution of language, mind and brain. Evolution of Communication 2: 45–116.Find this resource:

Givón, T. (2003). Bio‐linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Find this resource:

Givón, T. (2009). The genesis of syntactic complexity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (p. 666) Find this resource:

Givón, T. and Malle, B. F. (eds.) (2002). The evolution of language out of pre‐language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Find this resource:

Glass, L. and Mackey, M. C. (1988). From clocks to chaos. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Find this resource:

Glenberg, A. M. and Robertson, D. A. (1999). Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes 28: 1–26.Find this resource:

Glotin, H. (1995). La vie artificielle d'une société de robots parlants: Émergence et changement du code phonétique. Grenoble: DEA sciences cognitives‐Institut National Polytechnique de Grenoble.Find this resource:

Gogolev, A. I. (1993). Jakuty. Problemy ėtnogeneza i formirovanija kul'tury. [The Yakuts. Problems of their ethnogenesis and the formation of their culture] Yakutsk: Izdatel'stvo JaGU.Find this resource:

Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at work. The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Goldberg, A. E. (2008). Universal Grammar? Or prerequisites for natural language? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31: 522–523.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2003a). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2003b). The resilience of language: What gesture creation in deaf children can tell us about how all children learn language. New York: Psychology Press.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2005). What language creation in the manual modality tells us about the foundations of language. The Linguistic Review 22: 199–225.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S., Cook, S. W., and Mitchell, Z. A. (2009). Gesturing gives children new ideas about math. Psychological Science 20: 267–272.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S. and McNeill, D. (1999). The role of gesture and mimetic representation in making language the province of speech. In Corballis and Lea (eds.), 155–172.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S. and Mylander, C. (1998). Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures. Nature 91: 279–281.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S. and Sandhofer, C. (1999). Gestures convey substantive information about a child's thoughts to ordinary listeners. Developmental Science 2: 67–74.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S. and Singer, M. A. (2003). From children's hands to adults' ears: Gesture's role in teaching and learning. Developmental Psychology 39: 509–520.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S., Mylander, C., and Butcher, C. (1995). The resilience of combinatorial structure at the word level: Morphology in self‐styled gesture systems. Cognition 56: 195–262.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S., McNeill, D., and Singleton, J. (1996). Silence is liberating: Removing the handcuffs on grammatical expression in the manual modality. Psychological Review 103: 34–55.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D., and Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science 12: 516–522.Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S., Mylander, C., and Franklin, A. (2007). How children make language out of gesture: Morphological structure in gesture systems developed by American and Chinese deaf children. Cognitive Psychology 55: 87–135. (p. 667) Find this resource:

Goldin‐Meadow, S., So, W. C., Özyürek, A., and Mylander, C. (2008). The natural order of events: How speakers of different languages represent events nonverbally. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 105: 9163–9168.Find this resource:

Goldstein, L. (2003). Emergence of discrete gestures. In M. J. Solé, D. Recasens, and J. Romero (eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Barcelona: Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, 85–86.Find this resource:

Goldstein, L. and Fowler, C. A. (2003). Articulatory phonology: A phonology for public language use. In N. O. Schiller and A. S. Meyers (eds.), Phonetics and phonology in language comprehension and production. New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 159–207.Find this resource:

Goldstein, L., Byrd, D., and Saltzman, E. (2006). The role of vocal tract gestural action units in understanding the evolution of phonology. In Arbib (ed.), 215–249.Find this resource:

Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P., and West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 100: 8030–8035.Find this resource:

Goldstone, R. L. (1994). Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 123: 178–200.Find this resource:

Goldstone, R. L. (1998). Perceptual learning. Annual Review of Psychology 49: 585–612.Find this resource:

Gombrich, E. H. (1960). Art and illusion: A study in the psychology of pictorial representation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Find this resource:

Gómez, J. C. (2005). Requesting gestures in captive monkeys and apes: Conditioned responses or referential behaviours? Gesture 5: 91–105.Find this resource:

Gómez, R. L. (2002). Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science 13: 431–436.Find this resource:

Gómez, R. L. (2007). Statistical learning in infant language development. In M. G. Gaskell (ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 601–616.Find this resource:

Gómez, R. L. and Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1‐year‐olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition 70: 109–135.Find this resource:

Gómez, R. L. and Gerken, L. A. (2000). Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 178–186.Find this resource:

Good, C. D., Johnstrude, I., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N. A., Friston, K. J., and Frackowiak, R. S. J. (2001). Cerebral asymmetry and the effects of sex and handedness on brain structure: A voxel‐based morphometric analysis of 465 normal human brains. NeuroImage 14: 685–700.Find this resource:

Goodall, J. (1968). The behaviour of free‐living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs 1: 161–311.Find this resource:

Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Goodall, J. (1989). Gombe: Highlights and current research. In P. G. Heltne and L. A. Marquardt (eds.), Understanding chimpanzees. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2–21.Find this resource:

Goodman, M. (1996). Epilogue: A personal account of the origins of a new paradigm. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 5: 269–285.Find this resource:

Gopnik, M. and Crago, M. B. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition 39: 1–50.Find this resource:

Gordon, R. G. (2005). Ethnologue: Languages of the world. 15th Edition. Dallas, TX: Summer Institute of Linguistics. (p. 668) Find this resource:

Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual development and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Gould, J. L. and Marler, P. (1987). Learning by instinct. Scientific American 256: 74–85.Find this resource:

Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Gould, S. J. (1991). Exaptation: A crucial tool for evolutionary psychology. Journal of Social Issues 47: 43–65.Find this resource:

Gould, S. J. (1994). Eight little piggies. New York: W. W. Norton.Find this resource:

Gould, S. J. (1997). Evolution: The pleasures of pluralism. New York Review of Books, June 26.Find this resource:

Gould, S. J. (2002). The structure of evolutionary theory. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Gould, S. J. and Eldredge, N. (1977). Punctuated equilibria: The ‘tempo’ and ‘mode’ of evolution reconsidered. Paleobiology 3: 115–151.Find this resource:

Gould, S. J. and Lewontin, R. (1999). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist program. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 205: 581–598.Find this resource:

Gould, S. J. and Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation: A missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8: 4–15.Find this resource:

Graf Estes, K., Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., and Saffran, J. R. (2007). Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological Science 18: 254–260.Find this resource:

Grafton, S. T., Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography: 2. Observation compared with imagination. Experimental Brain Research 112: 103–111.Find this resource:

Grant, B. R. and Grant, P. R. (1996). Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwin's finches. Evolution 50: 2471–2487.Find this resource:

Grant, P. R. and Grant, B. R. (2002). Unpredictable evolution in a 30‐year study of Darwin's finches. Science 296: 707–711.Find this resource:

Grant, P. R. and Grant, B. R. (2006). Evolution of character displacement in Darwin's finches. Science 313: 224–226.Find this resource:

Graves, P. (1994). Flakes and ladders: What the archaeological record cannot tell us about the origins of language. World Archaeology 26: 158–171.Find this resource:

Green, R. E., Krause, J., Briggs, A. W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U., Kircher, M., Patterson, N., Li, H., Zhai, W., Fritz, M. H.‐Y., Hansen, N. F., Durand, E. Y., Malaspinas, A.‐S., Jensen, J. D., Marques‐Bonet, T., Alkan, C., Prüfer, K., Meyer, M., Burbano, H. A., Good, J. M., Schultz, R., Aximu‐Petri, A., Butthof, A., Höber, B., Höffner, B., Siegemund, M., Weihmann, A., Nusbaum, C., Lander, E. S., Russ, C., Novod, N., Affourtit, J., Egholm, M., Verna, C., Rudan, P., Brajkovic, D., Kucan, Z., Gušic, I., Doronichev, V. B., Golovanova, L. V., Lalueza‐Fox, C., de la Rasilla, M., Fortea, J., Rosas, A., Schmitz, R. W., Johnson, P. L. F., Eichler, E. E., Falush, D., Birney, E., Mullikin, J. C., Slatkin, M., Nielsen, R., Kelso, J., Lachmann, M., Reich, D., and Pääbo, S. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neanderthal genome. Science 328: 710–722.Find this resource:

Greenberg, J. H. (1966). Synchronic and diachronic universals on phonology. Language 42: 508–517.Find this resource:

Greenberg, J. H. (1969). Some methods of dynamic comparison in linguistics. In J. Puhvel (ed.), Substance and structure of language. Berkeley/Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press, 147–203. (p. 669) Find this resource:

Greenberg, J. H. (1978). How does a language acquire gender markers? In J. H. Greenberg, C. A. Ferguson, and E. Moravcsik (eds.), Universals of human language. Volume 3: Word structure. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 47–82.Find this resource:

Greenberg, J. H. (1992). Preliminaries to a systematic comparison between biological and linguistic evolution. In Hawkins and Gell‐Mann (eds.), 139–158.Find this resource:

Greenberg, J. H. (2000). Indo‐European and its closest relatives. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Find this resource:

Greenfield, P. M. (1991). Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behaviour. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14: 531–595 (with continuing commentary and a new response (1998), 21: 154–163).Find this resource:

Greenfield, P. M. and Savage‐Rumbaugh, E. S. (1990). Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: Processes of learning and invention in the development and evolution of language. In Parker and Gibson (eds.), 540–578.Find this resource:

Greenfield, P. M. and Savage‐Rumbaugh, E. S. (1991). Imitation, grammatical development, and the invention of protogrammar by an ape. In Krasnegor et al. (eds.), 235–258.Find this resource:

Greenfield, P. M. and Savage‐Rumbaugh, E. S. (1993). Comparing communicative competence in child and chimp: The pragmatics. Journal of Child Language 20: 1–26.Find this resource:

Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. Philosophical Review 66: 377–388.Find this resource:

Griffiths, P. E. (2002). What is innateness? The Monist 85: 70–85.Find this resource:

Griffiths, T. L. and Kalish, M. L. (2007). Language evolution by iterated learning with Bayesian agents. Cognitive Science 31: 441–480.Find this resource:

Grine, F. E. and Fleagle, J. G. (2009). The first humans: a summary perspective on the origin and early evolution of the Genus Homo. In Grine et al. (eds.), 197–207.Find this resource:

Grine, F. E., Fleagle, J. G., and Leakey, R. E. (eds.) (2009). The first humans: Origin and early evolution of the genus Homo. New York: Springer.Find this resource:

Groman, J. D. and Pellmyr, O. (2000). Rapid evolution and specialization following host colonization in yucca moth. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 13: 223–236.Find this resource:

Groves, C. P. (1989). A theory of primate and human evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Guilford, T. and Dawkins, M. S. (1991). Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Animal Behaviour 42: 1–14.Find this resource:

Güldemann, T. (2006). Structural isoglosses between Khoekhoe and Tuu: The Cape as a linguistic area. In Y. Matras, A. McMahon, and N. Vincent (eds.), Linguistic areas: convergence in historical and typological perspective. Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan, 99–134.Find this resource:

Güldemann, T. (2008). The Macro‐Sudan belt: Toward identifying a linguistic area in northern sub‐Saharan Africa. In B. Heine and D. Nurse (eds.), Africa as a Linguistic Area. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 151–185.Find this resource:

Güldemann, T. (2010). ‘Sprachraum’ and geography: Linguistic macro‐areas in Africa. In A. Lameli, R. Kehrein, and S. Rabanus (eds.), The handbook of language mapping. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 561–585.Find this resource:

Güldemann, T. and Elderkin, E. D. (2010). On external genealogical relationships of the Khoe family. In M. Brenzinger and C. König (eds.), Khoisan languages and linguistics: Proceedings of the 1st International Symposium January 4–8, 2003. Riezlern/Kleinwalsertal: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag. (p. 670) Find this resource:

Güldemann, T. and Vossen, R. (2000). Khoisan. In B. Heine and D. Nurse (eds.), African languages: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 99–122.Find this resource:

Gumperz, J. J. and Wilson, R. (1971). Convergence and creolization: A case from the Indo‐Aryan/Dravidian border in India. In D. Hymes (ed.), Pidginization and Creolization of Languages. Proceedings of a Conference held at the University of the West Indies, Mona, Jamaica, April 1968. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 151–167.Find this resource:

Gutiérrez, G., Sánchez, D., and Marín, A. (2002). A reanalysis of the ancient mitochondrial DNA sequences recovered from Neandertal bones. Molecular Biology and Evolution 19: 1359–1366.Find this resource:

Haile‐Selassie, Y., Suwa, G., and White, T. D. (2004). Late Miocene teeth from Middle Awash, Ethiopia, and early hominid dental evolution. Science 303: 1503–1505.Find this resource:

Hailman, J. P. (1969). How an instinct is learned. Scientific American 221: 98–106.Find this resource:

Haiman, J. (1994). Ritualization and the development of language. In W. Pagliuca (ed.), Perspectives on grammaticalization. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 3–28.Find this resource:

Haimoff, E. H. (1983). Occurrence of anti‐resonance in the song of the siamang (Hylobates syndactylus). American Journal of Primatology 5: 249–256.Find this resource:

Hames, R. B. (1988). The allocation of parental care among the Ye'kwana. In L. Betzig, M. Borgerhoff‐Mulder, and P. Turke (eds.), Human reproductive behaviour: A Darwinian perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 237–251.Find this resource:

Hamilton A. F. and Grafton, S. T. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. Journal of Neuroscience 26: 1133–1137.Find this resource:

Hamilton, C. R. and Vermeire, B. A. (1988). Complementary hemispheric specialization in monkeys. Science 242: 1691–1694.Find this resource:

Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7: 1–52.Find this resource:

Hammarström, H. (2007). Handbook of descriptive language knowledge: A full‐scale reference guide for typologists. Munich: Lincom Europa.Find this resource:

Hammerschmidt, K. and Fischer, J. (2008). Constraints in primate vocal production. In Oller and Griebel (2008b) (eds.), 93–119.Find this resource:

Hampe, B. (ed.) (2006). From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Find this resource:

Handford, P. (1988). Trill rate dialects in the rufous‐collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, in northwestern Argentina. Canadian Journal of Zoology 66: 2658–2670.Find this resource:

Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., van Schaik, C. P., and Wich, S. A. (2009a) Tool use in wild orang‐utans modifies sound production: a functionally deceptive innovation? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 276: 3689–3694.Find this resource:

Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., Singleton, I., Morrogh‐Bernard, H., Knott, C.D., Ancrenaz, M., Utami Atmoko, S. S., and Wich, S. A. (2009b). A description of the orangutan's vocal and sound repertoire, with a focus on geographical variation. In S. A. Wich, S. S. Utami Atmoko, T. Mitra Setia, and C. P. van Schaik (eds.), Orangutans: Geographical variation in behavioral ecology and conservation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 49–64.Find this resource:

Hare, B. and Tomasello, M. (1999). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology 113: 173–177.Find this resource:

Hare, B. and Tomasello, M. (2005). Human‐like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 439–444. (p. 671) Find this resource:

Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., and Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behavior 59: 771–785.Find this resource:

Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour 61: 139–151.Find this resource:

Harley, H. E., Putman, E. A., and Roitblat, H. L. (2003). Bottlenose dolphins perceive object features through echolocation. Nature 424: 667–669.Find this resource:

Harlow, J. M. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Publications of the Massachusetts Medical Society 2: 327–347.Find this resource:

Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42: 335–346.Find this resource:

Harnad, S. (2000). From sensorimotor praxis and pantomine to symbolic representations. The evolution of language. Proceedings of 3rd International Conference (EVOLANG 3) Paris, 118–125.Find this resource:

Harnad, S. (2005). To cognize is to categorize: Cognition is categorization. In Cohen and Lefebvre (eds.), 19–43.Find this resource:

Harnad, S. (2007). From knowing how to knowing that: Acquiring categories by word of mouth. Kaziemierz Naturalized Epistemology Workshop (KNEW), Kaziemierz, Poland, 2 September 2007.Find this resource:

Harnad, S. Steklis, H. D., and Lancaster. J. B. (eds.) (1976). Origins and evolution of language and speech. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280.Find this resource:

Harpending, H. C., Sherry, S. T., Rogers, A. R., and Stoneking, M. (1993). The genetic structure of ancient human populations. Current Anthropology 34: 483–496.Find this resource:

Harpending, H. C., Batzer, M. A., Gurven, M., Jorde, L. B., Rogers, A. R., and Sherry, S. T. (1998). Genetic traces of ancient demography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95: 1961–1967.Find this resource:

Harrison, R. M. and Nystrom, P. (2008). Handedness in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). Folia Primatologica 79: 253–268.Find this resource:

Hart, D. and Sussman, R. (2005). Man the hunted: Primates, predators and human evolution. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Find this resource:

Hasler, A. D. (1960). Guideposts of migrating fishes. Science 132: 785–792.Find this resource:

Haspelmath, M. (1999). Why is grammaticalization irreversible? Linguistics 37: 1043–1068.Find this resource:

Hasselquist, D., Bensch, S., and von Schantz, T. (1996). Correlation between male song repertoire, extra‐pair paternity and offspring survival in the great reed warbler. Nature 381: 229–232.Find this resource:

Hast, M. H., Fischer, J. M., Wetzel, A. B., and Thompson, V. E. (1974). Cortical motor representation of the laryngeal muscles in Macaca mulatta. Brain Research 73: 229–240.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. (1993). Right hemisphere dominance for the production of facial expression in monkeys. Science 261: 475–477.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. (2000). Wild minds: What animals really think. New York: Henry Holt.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. (2006). Moral minds. New York: Ecco Press.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. (2009a). Origin of the mind. Scientific American September 2009: 44–51.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. (2009b). The possibility of impossible cultures. Nature 460: 190–196.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. (2010). On obfuscation, obscurantism and opacity: Evolving conceptions of the faculty of language. In Larson et al. (eds.), 91–99.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. and Akre, K. (2001). Asymmetries in the timing of facial and vocal expressions by rhesus monkeys: Implications for hemispheric specialization. Animal Behaviour 61: 391–400. (p. 672) Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D. and Carey, S. (1998). Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: Evolutionary and developmental insights. In D. D. Cummins and C. Allen (eds.), The evolution of the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 51–106.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., and Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298: 1569–1579.Find this resource:

Hauser, M. D., Barner, D., and O'Donnell, T. (2007). Evolutionary linguistics: A new look at an old landscape. Language Learning and Development 3: 101–132.Find this resource:

Hawkes, K. (1990). Showing off: Tests of an hypothesis about men's foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology 12: 29–54.Find this resource:

Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., Blurton Jones, N. G., Alvarez, H., and Charnov, E. L. (1998). Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 95: 1336–1339.Find this resource:

Hawkins, J. A. (1994). A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Hawkins, J. A. (2004). Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Hawkins, J. A. and Gell‐Mann, M. (eds.) (1992). The evolution of human language. Proceedings of the Workshop on the Evolution of Human Languages, August 1989, Santa Fe, New Mexico. Santa Fe, NM: Addison‐Wesley Publishing Company.Find this resource:

Hawks, J., Hunley, K., Lee, S.‐H., and Wolpoff, M. (2000). Population bottlenecks and Pleistocene human evolution. Molecular Biology and Evolution 17: 2–22.Find this resource:

Hawks, J., Wang, E. T., Cochran, G. M., Harpending, H. C., and Moyzis, R. K. (2007). Recent acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 52: 20753–20758.Find this resource:

Hayes, C. (1952). The ape in our house. London: Gollancz.Find this resource:

Hayes, K. J. and Nissen, C. H. (1971). Higher mental functions of a home‐raised chimpanzee. In A. M. Schrier and F. Stollnitz (eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates: Modern research trends. New York: Academic Press, 59–115.Find this resource:

Hazy, T., Frank, M., and O'Reilly, R. (2006). Banishing the homunculus: Making working memory work. Neuroscience 139: 105–118.Find this resource:

Heath, J. (1978). Linguistic diffusion in Arnhem Land. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.Find this resource:

Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Find this resource:

Heffner, H. E. and Heffner, R. S. (1984). Temporal lobe lesions and perception of species‐specific vocalizations by macaques. Science 226: 75–76.Find this resource:

Heilbronner, P. L. and Holloway, R. L. (1988). Anatomical brain asymmetries in New World and Old World monkeys: Stages of temporal lobe development in primate evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 76: 39–48.Find this resource:

Heim, J. L. (1989). La nouvelle reconstitution du crane Néandertalien de La Chapelle‐Aux‐Saints: méthode et résultats. Bulletins et Mémoires de la Société d'Anthropologie de Paris 1: 95–118.Find this resource:

Heine, B. (1993). Auxiliaries: Cognitive forces and grammaticalization. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2002a). On the evolution of grammatical forms. In Wray (ed.), 376–397.Find this resource:

Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2002b). World lexicon of grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (p. 673) Find this resource:

Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2007). The genesis of grammar: A reconstruction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Heine, B. and Reh, M. (1984). Grammaticalization and reanalysis in African languages. Hamburg: Buske.Find this resource:

Heine, B., Claudi, U., and Hünnemeyer, F. (1991). Grammaticalization: A conceptual framework. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Henry, P. I., Morelli, G. A., and Tronick, E. Z. (2005). Child caretakers among Efe foragers of the Ituri Forest. In Hewlett and Lamb (eds.), 191–213.Find this resource:

Henshilwood, C. S. and Dubreuil, B. (2009). Reading the artefacts: Gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in Southern Africa. In Botha and Knight (eds.) (2009a), 41–61.Find this resource:

Henshilwood C. S. and Marean, C. W. (2003). The origin of modern human behaviour: Critique of the models and their test implications. Current Anthropology 44: 627–651.Find this resource:

Henshilwood, C. S., d'Errico, F., Yates, R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Duller, N. M., Sealy, J. C., Valladas, H., Watts, I., and Wintle, A. G. (2002). Emergence of modern human behaviour: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science 295: 1278–1280.Find this resource:

Henshilwood, C. S., d'Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., van Niekerk, K., and Jacobs, Z. (2004). Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa. Science 304: 404.Find this resource:

Herbinger, I., Papworth, S., Boesch, C., and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Vocal, gestural and locomotor responses of wild chimpanzees to familiar and unfamiliar intruders: A playback study. Animal Behaviour 78: 1389–1396.Find this resource:

Herman, L. M. (1980). Cognitive characteristics of dolphins. In L. M. Herman (ed.), Cetacean behavior: Mechanisms and functions. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 363–429.Find this resource:

Herman, L. M. (1987). Receptive competencies of language‐trained animals. In J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Beer, M.‐C. Busnel, and P. J. B. Slater (eds.), Advances in the study of behavior 17. Orlando, FL: Academic Press, 1–60.Find this resource:

Herman, L. M. (2002). Vocal, social, and self‐imitation by bottlenosed dolphins. In Dautenhahn and Nehaniv (eds.), 63–108.Find this resource:

Herman, L. M. and Forestell, P. H. (1985). Reporting presence or absence of named objects by a language‐trained dolphin. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 9: 667–681.Find this resource:

Herman, L. M. and Gordon, J. A. (1974). Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose dolphin. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21: 19–26.Find this resource:

Herman, L. M., Richards, D. G., and Wolz, J. P. (1984). Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins. Cognition 16: 129–219.Find this resource:

Herman, L. M., Kuczaj, S. A. II., and Holder, M. D. (1993). Responses to anomalous gestural sequences by a language‐trained dolphin: Evidence for processing of semantic relations and syntactic information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 122: 184–194.Find this resource:

Herman, L. M., Abichandani, S. L., Elhaji, A. N., Herman, E. Y. K., Sanchez, J. L., and Pack, A. A. (1999). Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referential character of the human pointing gesture. Journal of Comparative Psychology 111: 347–364.Find this resource:

Herrel, A., Huyghe, K., Vanhooydonck, B., Backeljau, T., Breugelmans, K., Grbac, I., Van Damme, R., and Irschick, D. J. (2008). Rapid large‐scale evolutionary divergence in morphology and performance associated with exploitation of a different dietary resource. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 105: 4792–4795.Find this resource:

Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez‐Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 317: 1360–1366. (p. 674) Find this resource:

Hershkovitz, P. (1977). Living New World monkeys. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Hess, E. (2008). Nim Chimpsky: The chimp who would be human. New York: Bantam.Find this resource:

Hewes, G. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology 14: 5–25.Find this resource:

Hewitt, G. P., MacLarnon, A. M., and Jones, K. E. (2002). The functions of laryngeal air sacs in primates: A new hypothesis. Folia Primatologica 73: 70–94.Find this resource:

Hewlett, B. S. and Lamb, M. E. (2005). Hunter‐gatherer childhoods. New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction Publishers.Find this resource:

Heyes, C. M. and Galef, B. G. (eds.) (1996). Social learning in animals: The roots of culture. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Hickey, R. (ed.) (2003). Motives for language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Hill, J. A. C. (1983). A computational model of language acquisition in the two‐year‐old. Cognition and Brain Theory 6: 287–317.Find this resource:

Hill, K. C. (ed.) (1979). The genesis of language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma Publishers.Find this resource:

Hilliard, A. T. and White, S. A. (2009). Possible precursors of syntactic components in other species. In Bickerton and Szathmáry (eds.), 161–183.Find this resource:

Hillix, W. A. and Rumbaugh, D. M. (2003). Animal bodies, human minds: Ape, dolphin, and parrot language skills. New York: Springer.Find this resource:

Hinde, R. A. (1966). Animal behavior: A synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology. New York: McGraw Hill.Find this resource:

Hock, H. H. (1991). Principles of historical linguistics. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Find this resource:

Hockett, C. F. (1958). In search of Jove's brow. American Speech 53: 243–313.Find this resource:

Hockett, C. F. (1960). Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. In W. E. Lanyon and W. N. Tavolga (eds.), Animal sounds and animal communication. Washington, DC: American Institute of Biological Sciences, 392–430.Find this resource:

Hoelzel, A. R. (1991). Killer whale predation on marine mammals at Punta Norte, Argentina; Food sharing, provisioning and foraging strategy. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 29: 197–204.Find this resource:

Hoit, J. D., Shea, S. A., and Banzett, R. B. (1994). Speech production during mechanical ventilation in tracheostomized individuals. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37: 53–63.Find this resource:

Holden, C. (2006). Random samples: Polly Pachyderm. Science 314: 29.Find this resource:

Holekamp, K. E., Sakai, S. T., and Lundrigan, B. L. (2007). Social intelligence in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 362: 523–538.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (1968). The evolution of the primate brain: Some aspects of quantitative relations. Brain research 7: 121–172.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (1969). Culture: A human domain. Current Anthropology 10: 395–412.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (1975). Role of social behavior in the evolution of the brain. 43rd James Arthur lecture. New York: American Museum of Natural History.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (1978). Problems of brain endocast interpretation and African hominid evolution. In C. Jolly (ed.), Early hominids of Africa. New York: St. Martin's Press, 379–401.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (1981). Culture, symbols and human brain evolution: A synthesis. Dialectical Anthropology 5: 287–303. (p. 675) Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (1983). Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behavior. Human Neurobiology 2: 105–114.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (1995). Toward a synthetic theory of human brain evolution. In Changeux and Chavallion (eds.), 42–60.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (1996). Evolution of the human brain. In Lock and Peters (eds.), 74–116.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. (2009). Brain fossils: Endocasts. In L. R. Squire (ed.), Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, Vol 2. Oxford: Academic Press, 353–361.Find this resource:

Holloway, R. L. and de Lacoste‐Lareymondie, M. C. (1982). Brain endocast asymmetry in pongids and hominids: Some preliminary findings on the paleontology of cerebral dominance. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 58: 101–110.Find this resource:

Holveck, M‐J., Viera de Castro, A. C., Lachlan, R. F., ten Cate, C., and Riebel, K. (2008). Accuracy of song syntax learning and singing consistency signal early condition in zebra finches. Behavioral Ecology 19: 1267–1281.Find this resource:

Hook‐Costigan, M. A. and Rogers, L. J. (1998). Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. Neuropsychologia 36: 1265–1273.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D. (1995). Hand preferences for a coordinated bimanual task in 110 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Cross‐sectional analysis. Journal of Comparative Psychology 109: 291–297.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Comparative and familial analysis of handedness in great apes. Psychological Bulletin 132: 538–559.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D. (ed.) (2007). Evolution of hemispheric specialization in primates. Oxford: Elsevier.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D. and Cantalupo, C. (2004). Handedness in chimpanzees is associated with asymmetries in the primary motor cortex but not with homologous language areas. Behavioral Neuroscience 118: 1176–1183.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D. and de Waal, F. B. M. (1995). Behavioral laterality in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus): Replication and extension. International Journal of Primatology 16: 261–276.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D. and Marino L. (2000). Asymmetries in cerebral width in nonhuman primate brains as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Neuropsychologia 38: 493–499.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D. and Nir, T. (2010). Planum temporale surface area and grey matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The effect of handedness and comparison with findings in humans. Behavioural Brain Research 208: 436–443.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D., Marino, L., Rilling, J. K., and MacGregor, L. A. (1998). Planum temporale asymmetries in Great Apes as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroreport 9: 2913–2918.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D., Pilcher, D. L., and MacGregor, L. A. (2000). Sylvian fissure length asymmetries in primates revisited: A comparative MRI study. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 56: 293–299.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., Freeman, H., Buehler, N., Reynolds, E., and Schapiro, S. (2005). The distribution and development of handedness for manual gestures in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Psychological Science 16: 487.Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., and Cantalupo, C. (2007a). Neuroanatomical correlates of handedness for tool use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Implication for theories on the evolution of language. Psychological Science 18: 971–977. (p. 676) Find this resource:

Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P., and Leavens, D. A. (2007b). Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Animal Behaviour 73: 281–286.Find this resource:

Hopper, P. J. (1998). Emergent grammar. In M. Tomasello (ed.), The new psychology of language. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 155–175.Find this resource:

Hopper, P. J. and Traugott, E. C. (2003). Grammaticalization. Second edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Hostetter, A. B., Cantero, M., and Hopkins, W. D. (2001). Differential use of vocal and gestural communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in response to the attentional status of a human (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology 115: 337–343.Find this resource:

Howell, F. C. (1951). The place of Neanderthal Man in human evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 9: 379–416.Find this resource:

Hrdy, S. B. (1976). Care and exploitation of nonhuman primate infants by conspecifics other than the mother. In J. S. Rosenblatt (ed.), Advances in the study of behavior 6. New York: Academic Press, 101–158.Find this resource:

Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother nature: Maternal instincts and how they shape the human species. New York: Random House.Find this resource:

Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Hublin, J.‐J. (1998). Climatic changes, paleogeography, and the evolution of the Neandertals. In T. Akazawa, K. Aoki, and O. Bar‐Yosef (eds.), Neandertals and modern humans in Western Asia. New York: Plenum Press, 295–310.Find this resource:

Hughes, C. and Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2‐ to 4‐years. Developmental Psychology 43: 1447–1459.Find this resource:

Hughes, C. and Ensor, R. (2008). Does executive function matter for preschoolers' problem behaviors? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 36: 1–14.Find this resource:

Hunt, G. R. and Gray, R. D. (2004). Direct observations of pandanus‐tool manufacture and use by a New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Animal Cognition 7: 114–120.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (1987). Language and number. Oxford: Blackwell.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (1989). Biological evolution of the Saussurean sign as a component of the language acquisition device. Lingua 77: 187–222.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (1990a). Beyond the roadblock in linguistic evolution studies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 736–737.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (1990b). Nativist and functional explanations in language acquisition. In I. M. Roca (ed.), Logical issues in language acquisition. Dordrecht: Foris, 85–136.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (2000). Social transmission favours linguistic generalization. In Knight et al. (eds.), 324–352.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (2002). The roles of expression and representation in language evolution. In Wray (ed.), 311–334.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (2003a). The language mosaic and its evolution. In Christiansen and Kirby (eds.), 38–57.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (2003b). The neural basis of predicate‐argument structure. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26: 261–283.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (2007). The origins of meaning: Language in the light of evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. (2011). The origins of grammar: Language in the light of evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (p. 677) Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R. and Dediu, D. (2009). Diversity in languages, genes, and the language faculty. In Botha and Knight (eds.) (2009a), 167–188.Find this resource:

Hurford, J. R., Studdert‐Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (eds.) (1998). Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Hurst, J. A., Baraitser, M., Auger, E., Graham, F., and Norel, S. V. (1990). An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 32: 352–355.Find this resource:

Hymes, D. (1971). Pidginization and creolization of languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

International Human Genome Sequencing Consortium (2004). Finishing the euchromatic sequence of the human genome. Nature 431: 931–945.Find this resource:

Irie‐Sugimoto, N., Kobayashi, T., Sato, T., and Hasegawa, T. (2008). Evidence of means–end behavior in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Animal Cognition 11: 359–365.Find this resource:

Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton‐Century‐Crofts.Find this resource:

Izard, C. E. (1980). Cross‐cultural perspectives on emotions and emotion communication. In H. C. Triandis and W. Lonner (eds.), Handbook of cross‐cultural psychology: Vol. 3 Basic Processes. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon, 185–222.Find this resource:

Jablonski, N. G. and Aiello, L. C. (eds.) (1998). The origin and diversification of language. San Francisco, CA: California Academy of Sciences.Find this resource:

Jackendoff, R. (1999). Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3: 272–279.Find this resource:

Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Jackendoff, R. (2010). Your theory of language evolution depends on your theory of language. In Larson et al. (eds.), 63–72.Find this resource:

Jackendoff, R. and Pinker, S. (2005). The nature of the language faculty and its implications for evolution of language (Reply to Fitch, Hauser, and Chomsky). Cognition 97: 211–225.Find this resource:

Jackson, A. P., Eastwood, H., Bell, S. M., Adu, J., Toomes, C., Carr, I. M., Roberts, E., Hampshire, D. J., Crow, Y. J., Mighell, A. J., Karbani, G., Jafri, H., Rashid, Y., Mueller, R. F., Markham, A. F., and Woods, C. G. (2002). Identification of microcephalin, a protein implicated in determining the size of the human brain. American Journal of Human Genetics 71: 136–42.Find this resource:

Jacobs, Z., Roberts, R. G., Galbraith, R. F., Deacon, H. J., Grün, R., MacKay, A., Mitchell, P., Vogelsang, R., and Wadley, L. (2008). Ages for the Middle Stone Age of Southern Africa: Implications for human behavior and dispersal. Science 322: 733–735.Find this resource:

Jacobsen, W. H., Jr. (1980). Inclusive/exclusive: A diffused pronominal category in native western North America. In J. Kreiman and A. E. Ojeda (eds.), Papers from the parasession on pronouns and anaphora. Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistics Society, 204–227.Find this resource:

Jäger, H., Baronchelli, A., Briscoe, E., Christiansen, M. H., Griffiths, T., Jäger, G., Kirby, S., Komarova, N., Richerson, P. J., Steels, L., and Triesch, J. (2009). What can mathematical, computational and robotic models tell us about the origins of syntax? In Bickerton and Szathmáry (eds.), 385–410.Find this resource:

Jakobson, R. (1967). About the relation between visual and auditory signs. In W. Wathen‐Dunn (ed.), Models for the perception of speech and visual form. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1–7. (p. 678) Find this resource:

Janda, R. D. (2001). Beyond ‘pathways’ and ‘unidirectionality’: On the discontinuity of language transmission and the counterability of grammaticalization. In L. Campbell (ed.), Grammaticalization: A critical assessment (Special issue of Language Sciences), 265–340.Find this resource:

Janik, V. M. (1999). Origins and implications of vocal learning in bottlenose dolphins. In Box and Gibson (eds.), 308–326.Find this resource:

Janik, V. M. (2000a). Food‐related bray calls in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 267: 923–927.Find this resource:

Janik, V. M. (2000b). Source levels and the estimated active space of bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) whistles in the Moray Firth, Scotland. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 186: 673–680.Find this resource:

Janik, V. M. (2000c). Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Science 289: 1355–1357.Find this resource:

Janik, V. M. (2005). Acoustic communication networks in marine mammals. In P. K. McGregor (ed.), Animal communication networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 390–415.Find this resource:

Janik, V. M. and Slater, P. J. B. (1997). Vocal learning in mammals. In P. J. B. Slater, C. T. Snowdon, J. S. Rosenblatt, and M. Milinkski (eds.), Advances in the study of behavior 26. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 59–99.Find this resource:

Janik, V. M., Sayigh, L. S., and Wells, R. S. (2006). Signature whistle contour shape conveys identity information to bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 103: 8293–8297.Find this resource:

Jarvis, E. D. (2004). Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016: 749–777.Find this resource:

Jarvis, E. D., Güntürkün, O., Bruce, L., Csillag, A., Karten, H. J., Kuenzel, W., Medina, L., Paxinos, G., Perkel, D. J., Shimizu, T., Striedter, G. F., Wild, M., Ball, G. F., Dugas‐Ford, J., Durand, S., Hough, G., Husband, S., Kubikova, L., Lee, D. W., Mello, C. V., Powers, A., Siang, C., Smulders, T. V., Wada, K., White, S. A., Yamamoto, K., Yu, J., Reiner, A., and Butler, A. B. (2005). Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate evolution. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 151–159.Find this resource:

Jenkins, L. (2000). Biolinguistics: Exploring the biology of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Jerison, H. J. (1973). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Jerison, H. J. (1982). Allometry, brain size, cortical surface, and convolutedness. In E. Armstrong and D. Falk (eds.), Primate brain evolution: Methods and concepts. New York: Plenum Press, 77–84.Find this resource:

Jespersen, O. (1895/1983). Progress in language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Find this resource:

Jobling, M. A. and Tyler‐Smith, C. (2003). The human Y chromosome: An evolutionary marker comes of age. Nature Reviews Genetics 4: 598–612.Find this resource:

Jobling, M. A., Hurles, M. E., and Tyler‐Smith, C. (2004). Human evolutionary genetics. New York: Garland Science.Find this resource:

Johanson, D. C., White, T. D., and Coppens, Y. (1978). A new species of the genus Australopithecus (Primates: Hominidae) from the Pliocene of East Africa. Kirtlandia 28: 1–14.Find this resource:

Johanson, L. (2002). Contact‐induced change in a code‐copying framework. In M. C. Jones and E. Esch (eds.), Language change: The interplay of internal, external and extra‐linguistic factors. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 285–313. (p. 679) Find this resource:

Johansson, S. (2005). Origins of language: Constraints on hypotheses. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Find this resource:

Johansson, S. (2008). Seeking compositionality in holistic protolanguage without substructure – do counterexamples overwhelm the fractionation process? In A. D. M. Smith et al. (eds.), 171–178.Find this resource:

Johnson‐Pynn, J., Fragaszy, D. M., Hirsh, E. M., Brakke, K. E., and Greenfield, P. M. (1999). Strategies used to combine seriated cups by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), and capuchins (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology 113: 37–48.Find this resource:

Journal of Human Evolution 37(3–4) (1999). Special issue devoted to the Pleistocene site of Gran Dolina, Sierra de Atapuerca, Spain, 309–700.Find this resource:

Jungers, W. L., Pokempner, A. A., Kay, R. F., and Cartmill, M. (2003). Hypoglossal canal size in living hominoids and the evolution of human speech. Human Biology 75: 473–484.Find this resource:

Jürgens, U. (1998). Neuronal control of mammalian vocalization, with special reference to the squirrel monkey. Naturwissenschaften 85: 376–388.Find this resource:

Jürgens, U. (2002). Neural pathways underlying vocal control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 26: 235–258.Find this resource:

Jürgens, U. and Schriever, S. (1991). Respiratory muscle activity during vocalization in the squirrel monkey. Folia Primatologia 56: 121–132.Find this resource:

Jusczyk, P. W. (1997). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Just, M. A. and Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review 99: 122–149.Find this resource:

Kako, E. (1999a). Elements of syntax in the systems of three language‐trained animals. Animal Learning and Behavior 27: 1–14.Find this resource:

Kako, E. (1999b). Response to Pepperberg; Herman and Uyeyama; and Shanker, Savage‐Rumbaugh, and Taylor. Animal Learning and Behavior 27: 26–27.Find this resource:

Kalish, M. L., Griffiths, T. L., and Lewandowsky, S. (2007). Iterated learning: Intergenerational knowledge transmission reveals inductive biases. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 14: 288–294.Find this resource:

Kaminski, J., Call, J., and Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for fast mapping. Science 304: 1682–1683.Find this resource:

Kane, M. J. and Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working‐memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual‐differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9: 637–671.Find this resource:

Kaplan, H. S., Hill, K., Lancaster, J. B., and Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Brains, learning and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology 9: 156–185.Find this resource:

Karmiloff, K. and Karmiloff‐Smith, A. (2001). Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Katz, J. J. (1976). The effability hypothesis. In Harnad et al. (eds.), 445–455.Find this resource:

Katz, P. A. (1963). Effects of labels on children's perception and discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 66: 423–428.Find this resource:

Kauffman, S. A. (1993). The origins of order. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Kawai, M. (1965). Newly‐acquired pre‐cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima islet. Primates 6: 1–30. (p. 680) Find this resource:

Kay, R. F., Ross, C., and Williams, B. A. (1997). Anthropoid origins. Science 275: 797–804.Find this resource:

Kay, R. F., Cartmill, M., and Balow, M. (1998). The hypoglossal canal and the origin of human vocal behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 95: 5417–5419.Find this resource:

Kayser, M., Brauer, S., Cordaux, R., Casto, A., Lao, O., Zhivotovsky, L. A., Moyse‐Faurie, C., Rutledge, R. B., Schiefenhoevel, W., Gil, D., Lin, A. A., Underhill, P. A., Oefner, P. J., Trent, R. J., and Stoneking, M. (2006). Melanesian and Asian origins of Polynesians: mtDNA and Y chromosome gradients across the Pacific. Molecular Biology and Evolution 23: 2234–2244.Find this resource:

Ke, J., Minett, J. W., Au, C.‐P., and Wang, W. S.‐Y. (2002). Self‐organization and selection in the emergence of vocabulary. Complexity 7: 41–54.Find this resource:

Ke, J., Ogura, M., and Wang, W. S.‐Y. (2003). Optimization models of sound systems using genetic algorithms. Computational Linguistics 29: 1–18.Find this resource:

Kegl, J., Senghas, A., and Coppola, M. (1999). Creation through contact: Sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua. In M. DeGraff (ed.), Language creation and language change: Creolization, diachrony, and development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 179–237.Find this resource:

Kelemen, G. (1969). Anatomy of the larynx and the anatomical basis of vocal performance. In G. H. Bourne (ed.), The chimpanzee. Basel: Karger, 165–186.Find this resource:

Keller, G. B. and Hahnloser, R. H. R. (2009). Neural processing of auditory feedback during vocal practice in a songbird. Nature 457: 187–190.Find this resource:

Keller, R. (1994). On language change: The invisible hand in language. London: Routledge.Find this resource:

Keller, S. S., Highley, J. R., Garcia‐Finana, M., Sluming, V., Rezaie, R., and Roberts, N. (2007). Sulcal variability, stereological measurement and asymmetry of Broca's area on MR images. Journal of Anatomy 211: 534–555.Find this resource:

Keller, S. S., Crow, T. J., Foundas, A. L., Amunts, K., and Roberts, N. (2009a). Broca's area: Nomenclature, anatomy, typology and asymmetry. Brain and Language 109: 29–48.Find this resource:

Keller, S. S., Roberts, N., and Hopkins, W. D. (2009b). A comparative Magnetic Resonance Imaging study of the anatomy, variability and asymmetry of Broca's area in the human and chimpanzee brain. Journal of Neuroscience 29: 14607–14616.Find this resource:

Kellogg, W. N. and Kellogg, L. A. (1933). The ape and the child: A comparative study of the environmental influence upon early behaviour. New York and London: Hafner Publishing Company.Find this resource:

Kempe, V. and Brooks, P. (2001). The role of diminutives in the acquisition of Russian gender: Can elements of child‐directed speech aid in learning morphology? Language Learning 51: 221–256.Find this resource:

Kendon, A. (1995). Gestures as illocutionary and discourse structure markers in Southern Italian conversation. Journal of Pragmatics 23: 247–279.Find this resource:

Khanina, O. (No date). A case‐study in historical sociolinguistics: Reconstructing the pattern of multilingualism of Enets (1850‐1950). Ms.Find this resource:

Kier, W. M. and Smith, K. K. (1985). Tongues, tentacles and trunks: The biomechanics of movement in muscular‐hydrostats. Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society 83: 307–324.Find this resource:

Kihm, A. (2002). Langues créoles et origine du langage: État de la question. Langages 146: 59–69.Find this resource:

Kilian, A., von Fersen, L., and Güntürkün, O. (2000). Lateralization of visuospatial processing in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Brain Research 116: 211–215. (p. 681) Find this resource:

Kilian, A., von Fersen, L., and Güntürkün, O. (2005). Left hemispheric advantage for numerical abilities in the bottlenose dolphin. Behavioural Processes 68: 179–184.Find this resource:

Kimbel, W. H. (2009). The origin of Homo. In Grine et al. (eds.), 31–37.Find this resource:

Kimbel, W. H., Walter, R. C., Johanson, D. C., Reed, K. E., Aronson, J. L., Assefa, Z., Marean, C. W., Eck, G. G., Bobe, R., Hovers, E., Rak, Y., Vondra, C., Yemane, T., York, D., Chen, Y., Evensen, N. M., and Smith, P. E. (1996). Late Pliocene Homo and Oldowan tools for the Hadar formation (Kada Hadar member) Ethiopia. Journal of Human Evolution 31: 549–561.Find this resource:

Kimbel, W. H., Johanson, D. C., and Rak, Y. (1997). Systematic assessment of a maxilla of Homo from Hadar, Ethiopia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 103: 235–262.Find this resource:

Kinderman, P., Dunbar, R. I. M., and Bentall, R. P. (1998). Theory‐of‐mind deficits and causal attributions. British Journal of Psychology 89: 191–204.Find this resource:

Kinsella, A. (2009). Language evolution and syntactic theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Kirby, S. (1999). Function, selection and innateness: The emergence of language universals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Kirby, S. (2000). Syntax without natural selection: How compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In Knight et al. (eds.), 303–323.Find this resource:

Kirby, S. (2001). Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure: An iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation 5: 102–110.Find this resource:

Kirby, S. (2002). Natural language from artificial life. Artificial Life 8: 185–215.Find this resource:

Kirby, S. and Hurford, J. R. (2002). The emergence of linguistic structure: An overview of the iterated learning model. In Cangelosi and Parisi (eds.), 121–148.Find this resource:

Kirby, S., Smith, K., and Brighton, H. (2004). From UG to universals: Linguistic adaptation through iterated learning. Studies in Language 28: 587–607.Find this resource:

Kirby, S., Dowman, M., and Griffiths, T. L. (2007). Innateness and culture in the evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104: 5241–5245.Find this resource:

Kirby, S., Cornish, H., and Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 105: 10681–10686.Find this resource:

Kirby, S., Christiansen, M. H., and Chater, N. (2009). Syntax as an adaptation to the learner. In Bickerton and Szathmáry (eds.), 325–343.Find this resource:

Kirby, S., Smith, A., and Smith, K. (In preparation). Evolving language: Exploring the evolution of communication through working models.Find this resource:

Kiriazis, J. and Slobodchikoff, C. N. (2006). Perceptual specificity in the alarm calls of Gunnison's prairie dogs. Behavioural Processes 73: 29–35.Find this resource:

Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., and Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition 83: B35–B42.Find this resource:

Kirschner, M. W. and Gerhart, J. (2005). The plausibility of life: Resolving Darwin's dilemma. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Find this resource:

Kita, S. (2000). How representational gestures help speaking. In McNeill (ed.), 162–185.Find this resource:

Klein, R. G. (1989). The human career: Human biological and cultural origins. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Klein, R. G. (1992). The archaeology of modern human origins. Evolutionary Anthropology 1: 5–14. (p. 682) Find this resource:

Klein, R. G. (1998). Why anatomically modern people did not disperse from Africa 100,000 years ago. In T. Akazawa, K. Aoki, and O. Bar‐Yosef (eds.), Neanderthals and modern humans in Western Asia. New York: Plenum, 509–522.Find this resource:

Klein, R. G. (2003). Whither the Neanderthals? Science 299: 1525–1527.Find this resource:

Klein, R. G. and Edgar, B. (2002). The dawn of human culture. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Find this resource:

Klein, W. and Perdue, C. (1992). Utterance structure: Developing grammars. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Find this resource:

Klein, W. and Perdue, C. (1997). The Basic Variety (or: Couldn't natural languages be much simpler?). Second Language Research 13: 301–347.Find this resource:

Klima, E. and Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Knecht, S., Drager, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Floel, A., Ringelstein, E. B., and Henningsen, H. (2000). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain 123: 2512–2518.Find this resource:

Knight, A., Underhill, P. A., Mortensen, H. M., Zhivotovsky, L. A., Li, A. A., Henn, B. M., Louis, D., Ruhlen, M., and Mountain, J. L. (2003). African Y chromosome and mtDNA divergence provides insight into the history of click languages. Current Biology 13: 464–473.Find this resource:

Knight, C. (2008). Early human kinship was matrilineal. In N. J. Allen, H. Callan, R. I. M. Dunbar, and W. James (eds.), Early human kinship. Oxford: Blackwell, 61–82.Find this resource:

Knight, C. (2009). Language, ochre and the rule of law. In Botha and Knight (eds.) (2009a), 281–303.Find this resource:

Knight, C., Studdert‐Kennedy, M., and Hurford, J. R. (eds.) (2000). The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Knörnschild, M., Behr, O., and von Helversen, O. (2006). Babbling behavior in the sac‐winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata). Naturwissenschaften 93: 451–454.Find this resource:

Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, C. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297: 846–848.Find this resource:

Koops, K., McGrew, W. C., and Matsuzawa, T. (2010). Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) use cleavers and anvils to fracture Treculia africana fruits? Preliminary data on a new form of percussive technology. Primates 51: 175–178.Find this resource:

Krasnegor, N. A., Rumbaugh, D. M., Schiefelbusch, R. L., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (eds.) (1991). Biological and behavioral determinants of language development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Find this resource:

Krause, J., Lalueza‐Fox, C., Orlando, L., Enard, W., Green, R. E., Burbano, H. A., Hublin, J.‐J., Hanni, C., Fortea, J., de la Rasilla, M., Bertranpetit, J., Rosas, A., and Pääbo, S. (2007). The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals. Current Biology 17: 1908–1912.Find this resource:

Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., and Gottesman, R. F. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In McNeill (ed.), 261–283.Find this resource:

Krebs, J. R. and Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: Mind‐reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies (eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, 380–402.Find this resource:

Krieg, W. J. S. (1954). Connections of the frontal lobe of the monkey. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. (p. 683) Find this resource:

Kroodsma, D. E. (2005). The singing life of birds. New York: Houghton Mifflin.Find this resource:

Kroodsma, D. E., Houlihan, P. W., Fallon, P. A., and Wells, J. A. (1997). Song development in grey catbirds. Animal Behaviour 54: 457–464.Find this resource:

Kuczaj, S. A. II and Walker, R. T. (2006). How do dolphins solve problems? In E. A. Wasserman and T. R. Zentall (eds.), Comparative cognition: Experimental explorations of animal intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 580–601.Find this resource:

Kuhl, P. K. (1981). Discrimination of speech by nonhuman animals: Basic auditory sensitivities conducive to the perception of speech‐sound categories. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 70: 340–349.Find this resource:

Kuhl, P. K. (1987). The special mechanisms debate in speech research: Categorization tests on animals and infants. In S. Harnad (ed.) Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 355–386.Find this resource:

Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevikova, E. V., Rysinka, V. L., Stolyarova, E. I., Sundberg, U., and Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross‐language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science 277: 684–686.Find this resource:

Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., and Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign‐language experience in infancy: Effects of short‐term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 100: 9096–9101.Find this resource:

Kuhn, S. L. and Stiner, M. C. (2006). What's a mother to do: The division of labor among Neandertals and modern humans in Eurasia. Current Anthropology 47: 953–980.Find this resource:

Kuhn S. L. and Stiner M. C. (2007). Body ornamentation as information technology: Towards an understanding of the significance of early beads. In Mellars et al. (eds.), 45–54.Find this resource:

Kumar, S., Filipski, A., Swarna, V., Walker, A., and Hedges, S. B. (2005). Placing confidence limits on the molecular age of the human–chimpanzee divergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 102: 18842–18847.Find this resource:

Kuteva, T. (2001). Auxiliation: An enquiry into the nature of auxiliation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Kuteva, T.(2010). Are there languages without grammar? E-publication proceedings: SICOL-2010.Find this resource:

Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1958). Corticobulbar connexions to the pons and lower brain‐stem in man: An anatomical study. Brain 10: 371–375.Find this resource:

Lachlan, R. F. and Slater, P. J. B. (1999). The maintenance of vocal learning by gene‐culture interaction: The cultural trap hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 266: 701–706.Find this resource:

Ladd, D. R., Dediu, D., and Kinsella, A. R. (2008). Language and genes: Reflections on biolinguistics and the nature‐nurture question. Biolinguistics 2: 114–126.Find this resource:

Ladefoged, P. (1982). A course in phonetics. San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Find this resource:

Ladefoged, P. (2006). Features and parameters for different purposes. http://www.linguistics.ucla.edu/people/ladefoge/PLfeaturesParameters.pdf

Ladefoged, P. and Maddieson, I. (1996). The sounds of the world's languages. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Find this resource:

Lai, C. S., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha‐Khadem, F., and Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead‐domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413: 519–523.Find this resource:

Lai, C. S., Gerelli, D., Monaco, A. P., Fisher, S. E., and Copp, A. J. (2003). FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain 126: 2455–2462. (p. 684) Find this resource:

Laitman, J. T., Heimbuch, R. C., and Crelin, E. S. (1979). The basicranium of fossil hominids as an indicator of their upper respiratory systems. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 51: 15–34.Find this resource:

Laland, K. N. and Brown, G. R. (2002). Sense and nonsense. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Find this resource:

Laland, K. N., Odling‐Smee, J., and Feldman, M. W. (2001). Cultural niche construction and human evolution. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 14: 22–33.Find this resource:

Lancaster, J. B. and Kaplan, H. S. (2007). Chimpanzee and human intelligence: Life history, diet and the mind. In S. W. Gangestad and J. A. Simpson (eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies. New York: Guilford Press, 111–118.Find this resource:

Lancaster J. B. and Lancaster, C. S. (1983). Parental investment: The hominid adaptation. In D. Ortner (ed.), How humans adapt: A biocultural odyssey. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press, 33–66.Find this resource:

Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar, Vol. 1. Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Find this resource:

Laporte, M. N. C. and Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Animal Behaviour 80: 467–473.Find this resource:

Larson, R. K., Déprez, V., and Yamakido, H. (eds.) (2010). The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Leakey, L. S. B. (1959). A new fossil skull from Olduvai. Nature 184: 491–493.Find this resource:

Leakey, L. S. B., Tobias, P. V., and Napier, J. R. (1964). A new species of the genus Homo from Olduvai Gorge. Nature 202: 7–9.Find this resource:

Leakey, M. G., Feibel, C. S., McDougall, I., and Walker, A. (1995). New four‐million‐year‐old hominid species from Kanapoi and Allia Bay, Kenya. Nature 376: 565–571.Find this resource:

Leakey, M. G., Spoor, F., Brown, F. H., Gathogo, P. N., Kiarie, C., Leakey, L. N., and McDougall, I. (2001). New hominin genus from eastern Africa shows diverse middle Pliocene lineages. Nature 410: 433–440.Find this resource:

Leavens, D. A., Hostetter, A. B., Wesley, M. J., and Hopkins, W. D. (2004). Tactical use of unimodal and bimodal communication by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour 67: 467–476.Find this resource:

Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., and Bard, K. A., (2005). Understanding the point of chimpanzee pointing: Epigenesis and ecological validity. Current Directions in Psychological Science 14: 185–189.Find this resource:

Lee, C. E. (1999). Rapid and repeated invasions of fresh water by the copepod Eurytemora affinis. Evolution 53: 1423–1434.Find this resource:

Leger, D. W. (2005). First documentation of combinatorial song syntax in a suboscine passerine species. The Condor 107: 765–774.Find this resource:

Lehmann, C. (1982/1995). Thoughts on grammaticalization. Munich: Lincom Europa.Find this resource:

Lehmann, J., Korstjens, A. H., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Animal Behaviour 74: 1617–1629.Find this resource:

Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz's theory of instinctive behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology 28: 337–363.Find this resource:

Leitão, A., ten Cate, C., and Riebel, K. (2006). Within‐song complexity in a songbird is meaningful to both male and female receivers. Animal Behaviour 71: 1289–1296.Find this resource:

Lemasson, A. and Hausberger, M. (2004). Patterns of vocal sharing and social dynamics in a captive group of Campbell's monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli campbelli). Journal of Comparative Psychology 118: 347–359. (p. 685) Find this resource:

Lemasson, A., Gautier, J. P. and Hausberger, M. (2003). Vocal similarities and social bonds in Campbell's monkey (Cercopithecus campbelli). Comptes Rendus Biologies 326: 1185–1193.Find this resource:

Lemasson, A., Hausberger, M., and Zuberbühler, K. (2005). Socially meaningful vocal plasticity in adult Campbell's monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli). Journal of Comparative Psychology 119: 220–229.Find this resource:

LeMay, M. (1977). Asymmetries of the skull and handedness. Journal of Neurological Sciences 32: 243–253.Find this resource:

LeMay, M. (1985). Asymmetries of the brains and skulls of nonhuman primates. In S. D. Glick (ed.), Cerebral lateralization in nonhuman species. New York: Academic Press, 223–245.Find this resource:

LeMay, M. and Geschwind, N. (1975). Hemispheric differences in the brains of great apes. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 11: 48–52.Find this resource:

Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Riley.Find this resource:

Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Find this resource:

Lewontin, R. C. (1974). The analysis of variance and the analysis of causes. American Journal of Human Genetics 26: 400–411.Find this resource:

Lewontin, R. C. (1998). The evolution of cognition: Questions we will never answer. In D. Scarborough, S. Sternberg, and D. N. Osherson (eds.), An invitation to cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 107–132.Find this resource:

Lewontin, R. C. (2000). The triple helix: Gene, organism, and environment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Liberman, A. M. and Mattingly, I. G. (1989). A specialization for speech perception. Science 243: 489–494.Find this resource:

Liberman, A. M., Cooper F. S., Shankweiler, D. P., and Studdert‐Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review 74: 431–461.Find this resource:

Libert, A. (2000). A priori artificial languages. Munich: Lincom Europa.Find this resource:

Lickliter, R. (2008). The growth of developmental thought: Implications for a new evolutionary psychology. New Ideas in Psychology 26: 353–367.Find this resource:

Liebal, K., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2004). Use of gesture sequences in chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 64: 377–396.Find this resource:

Liebal, K., Pika, S., and Tomasello, M. (2006). Gestural communication in orang‐utans. Gesture 6: 1–38.Find this resource:

Lieber, R. (1992). Deconstructing morphology: Word formation in syntactic theory. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Lieberman, D. E. (2008). Speculations about the selective basis for modern human craniofacial form. Evolutionary Anthropology 17: 55–68.Find this resource:

Lieberman, D. E. and McCarthy, R. C. (1999). The ontogeny of cranial base angulation in humans and chimpanzees and its implications for reconstructing pharyngeal dimensions. Journal of Human Evolution 36: 487–517.Find this resource:

Lieberman, D. E., McCarthy, R. C., Hiiemae, K. M., and Palmer, J. B. (2001). Ontogeny of postnatal hyoid and larynx descent in humans. Archives of Oral Biology 46: 117–128.Find this resource:

Lieberman, D. E., McBratney, B. M., and Krovitz, G. (2002). The evolution and development of cranial form in Homo sapiens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 99: 1134–1139.Find this resource:

Lieberman, E., Michel, J., Jackson, J., Tang, T., and Nowak, M. (2007). Quantifying the evolutionary dynamics of language. Nature 449: 713–716. (p. 686) Find this resource:

Lieberman, P. (1980). On the development of vowel production in young children. In G. H. Yeni‐Komshian, J. F. Kavanagh, and C. A. Ferguson (eds.), Child phonology, perception and production. Volume 1: Production. New York: Academic Press, 113–142.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P. (1991). Uniquely human: The evolution of speech, thought, and selfless behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P. (2000). Human language and our reptilian brain. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P. (2002). On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 45: 36–62.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P. (2006). Toward an evolutionary biology of language. Cambridge, MA and London: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P. (2007). The evolution of speech: Its anatomical and neural bases. Current Anthropology 48: 39–66.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P. and Crelin, E. S. (1971). On the speech of Neanderthal man. Linguistic Inquiry 2: 203–222.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P., Crelin, E. S., and Klatt, D. H. (1972). Phonetic ability and the related anatomy of the newborn and adult human, Neanderthal man, and the chimpanzee. American Anthropologist 74: 287–307.Find this resource:

Lieberman, P., Laitman, J. T., Reidenberg, J. S., and Gannon, P. J. (1992). The anatomy, physiology, acoustics and perception of speech: Essential elements in analysis of the evolution of human speech. Journal of Human Evolution 23: 447–467.Find this resource:

Lightfoot, D. W. (1979). Principles of diachronic syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Lightfoot, D. W. (1991). Subjacency and sex. Language and Communication 11: 67–69.Find this resource:

Lightfoot, D. W. (1999). The development of language: Acquisition, change and evolution. Oxford: Blackwell.Find this resource:

Lightfoot, D. W. (2000). The spandrels of the linguistic genotype. In Knight et al. (eds.), 231–247.Find this resource:

Lightfoot, D. W. (2006). How new languages emerge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Liljencrants, J. and Lindblom, B. (1972). Numerical simulations of vowel quality systems. Language 48: 839–862.Find this resource:

Lin, J.‐W. (2005). Time in a language without tense: The case of Chinese. Journal of Semantics 23: 1–53.Find this resource:

Lindblom, B. (1986). Phonetic universals in vowel systems. In J. J. Ohala and J. J. Jaeger (eds.), Experimental phonology. Orlando, FL: Academic Press, 13–44.Find this resource:

Lindblom, B. (1998). Systemic constraints and adaptive change in the formation of sound structure. In Hurford et al. (eds.), 242–264.Find this resource:

Lindblom, B. (2011). Evolution of phonology. In P. C. Hogan (ed.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of the language sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Find this resource:

Lindblom, B. and Maddieson, I. (1988). Phonetic universals in consonant systems. In L. M. Hyman and C. N. Li (eds.), Language, speech and mind. London: Routledge, 62–78.Find this resource:

Lindblom, B., MacNeilage, P. F., and Studdert‐Kennedy, M. (1984). Self‐organizing processes and the explanation of language universals. In M. Butterworth, B. Comrie, and Ö (p. 687) . Dahl (eds.), Explanations for language universals. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter and Co., 181–203.Find this resource:

Lindblom, B., Diehl, R., Park, S.‐H., and Salvi, G. (In press). Sound systems are shaped by their users: The recombination of phonetic substance. In G. N. Clements and R. Ridouane (eds.), Where do phonological contrasts come from? Cognitive, physical and developmental bases of phonological features. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Find this resource:

Liu, H.‐M., Kuhl, P. K., and Tsao, F.‐M. (2003). An association between mothers' speech clarity and infants' speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science 6: F1–F10.Find this resource:

Liu, S. T. and Phillips, K. A. (2009). Sylvian fissure asymmetries in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Laterality 14: 217–227.Find this resource:

Liu, W., Jin, C., Zhang, Y., Cai, Y., Xing, S., Wu, X., Cheng, H., Edwards, R. L., Pan, W., Qin, D., An, Z., Trinkaus, E., and Wu, X. (2010). Human remains from Zhirendong, South China, and modern human emergence in East Asia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 107: 19201–19206.Find this resource:

Lock, A. and Peters, C. R. (eds.) (1996). Handbook of human symbolic evolution. New York: Clarendon Press.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic Press.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (1993). The child's path to spoken language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (1998). Social sound‐making as a precursor to spoken language. In Hurford et al. (eds.), 190–201.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2001a). First communion: The emergence of vocal relationships. Social Development 10: 294–308.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2001b). Vocal development in the human infant: functions and phonetics. In N. Hewlett, L. Kelly, and F. Windsor (eds.), Themes in clinical linguistics and phonetics. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 243–256.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2001c). Rank and relationships in the evolution of spoken language. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 7: 37–50.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2004). How do infants come to control the organs of speech? In B. Maassen, R. D. Kent, H. F. M. Peters, P. H. H. M. van Lieshout, and W. Hulstijn (eds.), Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 175–190.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2006). Parental selection of vocal behavior: Crying, cooing, babbling and the evolution of language. Human Nature 17: 155–168.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2008a). Cost and complexity: Selection for speech and language. Journal of Theoretical Biology 25: 640–652.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2008b). The trait of human language: Lessons from the canal boat children of England. Biology and Philosophy 23: 347–361.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2009). Evolutionary developmental linguistics: Naturalization of the faculty of language. Language Sciences 31: 33–59.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. (2010). The development of linguistic systems: Insights from evolution. In J. Guendouzi, F. Loncke, and M. J. Williams (eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistic and cognitive processes: Perspectives in communication disorders. London: Psychology Press, 3–29.Find this resource:

Locke, J. L. and Bogin, B. (2006). Language and life history: A new perspective on the evolution and development of linguistic communication. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 259–325.Find this resource:

Lombard, M. and Phillipson, L. (2010). Indications of bow and stone‐tipped arrow use 64 000 years ago in KwaZulu‐Natal, South Africa. Antiquity 84: 635–648. (p. 688) Find this resource:

Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Find this resource:

Lorenzo, G. and Longa, V. M. (2003). Homo loquens: Biologia y evolucion del lenguaje. Lugo: Tris Tram.Find this resource:

Losin, E. R., Freeman, H., Russell, J. L., Meguerditchian, A., and Hopkins, W. D. (2008). Left hemisphere specialization for oro‐facial movements of learned vocal signals by captive chimpanzees. Plos ONE 3: 1–7.Find this resource:

Losos, J. B., Warheit, K. I., and Schoener, T. W. (1997). Adaptive differentiation following experimental island colonizations in Anolis lizards. Nature 387: 70–73.Find this resource:

Lou, H., Luber, B., Crupain, M., Keenan, J. P., Nowak, M., and Kjaer, T. (2004). Who am I? On the nature of subjectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 101: 6827–6832.Find this resource:

Lupyan, G. (2006). Labels facilitate learning of novel categories. In Cangelosi et al. (eds.), 190–197.Find this resource:

Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.Find this resource:

Luria, A. R., Tsvetkova, L. S., and Futer, D. S. (1965). Journal of Neurological Science 2: 288–292.Find this resource:

Lyon, B. E., Eadie, J. M., and Hamilton, L. D. (1994). Parental choice selects for ornamental plumage in American coot chicks. Nature 37: 240–243.Find this resource:

Lyon C., Nehaniv C. L., and Cangelosi A. (eds.) (2007). Emergence of communication and language. London: Springer.Find this resource:

Macaulay, V., Hill, C., Achilli, A., Rengo, C., Clarke, D., Meehan, W., Blackburn, J., Semino, O., Scozzari,